Episode Transcript
Available transcripts are automatically generated. Complete accuracy is not guaranteed.
Speaker 1 (00:03):
Welcome to Stuff to Blow your Mind from how Stuff
Works dot com. Hey, welcome to Stuff to Blow your Mind.
My name is Robert Lamb and I'm Julie Douglas. Now Julie.
Normally we don't we don't do a lot of readings
from the Bible on this podcast, but I've got a
couple here I want to hit everyone with because of
Ravens what we're talking about. Both of these are from
(00:25):
the Book of Matthew, the New Testament and the first
one Matthew eleven. At that time, Jesus explained, I give
praise for you, Father, Lord of Heaven and Earth, for
although you have hidden these things from the wise and
the learning, you have revealed them to the little ones.
The little ones, little being the children. And then there's
another one where he says, I tell you the truth.
Unless you change and become like little children, you will
(00:47):
never enter the Kingdom of Heaven. And I think there's
a little more there about needles and rich people. But
needles and rich people, yeah, yeah, Like it's easier for
a camel to go through the eyeban needle than for
a rich man. And can all you know, lots of
the you know, New testament wisdom that Jesus is spouting
and in this book. But these two passages in particular
interesting because they both talk about the this childhood nature,
(01:10):
this there's something special about children that enables them to
see the world as it really is, to to see
through the grown up bs and and get at the
truth of the matter. And it's something you see, I
mean throughout the human history. I mean you see it
a lot in fiction Chronicles of Narnia, Susan becomes too sophisticated, well,
(01:31):
too grown up for Narnia, can't go there anymore. Puff
the magic Dragon, as you remember a little Jackie Paper
Paper reaches the point where you can't see Narnia the
children and Stephen King's it you too. In order to
defeat this evil clown shifting monster, you have to have
this spirit of a child. Yeah. And my neighbor Totoro,
the farest spirits, adorable as they are, he's got to
(01:53):
be a child to see them. And and so then
there's of course this longstanding thing like that. There's the
whole kids say that darned as things right, where like
Bill Cosby show, wasn't it it was? Yeah? And I
think it was. It was an older show even before
Cosby took it over, but he's the one most remembered
for it. But the idea being that you bring a
kid on and you just let them talk, and they're
you're just gonna drop truth bombs on you because they
(02:15):
don't know any better. Though, I found it interesting that
Bill Cosby is also quoted as saying it's saying quote,
a person with no children says, well, I just love children.
And you say why, and they say, because the child
is so truthful, and that's what I love about them.
They tell the truth. And Cosby goes on to say,
but that's a lie. I've got five of them and
the only time they tell the truth is when they're
having pain. Even that sometimes can be a lie, as
(02:37):
I have discovered. Yeah, you're you're a mom. What's your
take on the the innocence and the truth bombs of children?
The truth bombs? Um, Well, I mean I think everything
is is new to them, right, so immediately kids see
things an entirely different way because they're piecing together context
and being able to see things, um from an entirely
(02:58):
new perspective, as we know, is a completely liberating thing.
And there's a power to that, and we've talked about
that before that as we age, we tend to let
some of that uh slaw off of us, right, because
we're so used to sort of establishing a pattern, going
with that and then moving forward. But to paraphrase Picasso,
he said he took his whole life to think like
(03:20):
a child artistically to reach that place where he could
once again delve into novel ideas or novel representations of
the human experience. And in fact, I am looking at
Picasso right now because in our podcast Booth. I don't
know if we've ever mentioned this before. I don't think
you have not in the podcast. Uh well, we have
(03:41):
a couple of photos in the podcast Booth, and the
one that I get to stare at is Picasso in
his underwear. So I think about this idea a lot,
so that I don't think about Picasso in his underwear
with a bat for a while. But then then someone
moved it. Yeah I did. I covered up his private
parts in this one. They did. Um, But there is
this I think he puts a you well that you
know it takes a lot of effort to try to
(04:03):
bust out of these constraints that we because we have
to we placed in our lives, um, and to be
able to think in a way that is completely mind
blowing a new Yeah. I mean there is something about
the creativity alone of a child. UM. I think I've
mentioned this before. There's a in Atlanta. There's a there's
an improv company called Dad's Garage and they do if
(04:25):
you go at night, you've often get a lot of
very blue material from these improv actors. They're getting up there.
They're blooming dirty dirty. Yeah, they're doing free association and
it's and all sorts of outlandish things are coming up
that I couldn't even mention on the podcast, but they
would all. They also do a show and I think
they still do it called Uncle Grahampa's Hoodaily story Time.
(04:46):
And in this show, it's the same improv actors, these
same same guys and gals that are just really tearing
it up at night with ranching material. But now they're
in the a m and they have they're performing to
an audience of mostly children, but they're also doing improv
and they're getting tips from the audience, so they'll they'll
ask the kids in the audience, Hey, what should the
name of this princess be and what should the story
(05:07):
that we tell be called? And uh, I've been to
it a couple of times, and there it is always
amazing because from these children, they're able to come up
with the craziest ideas, like stuff that these these these
experienced and highly creative improv actors would never be able
to pull out. Like like I remember one when when
they asked what should the princess be called, the little
(05:27):
girl said that the princess name should be quote Batman
the Girl, which is incredible. You know, it's like that
kind of like strange, free association that you're just not
going to get from an adult. Well, yeah, I remember
seeing this Ted talk and uh, I'll show this up
on Facebook or in a post. I don't remember the
name of the Ted talk right now, but they were
starting they were talking about accessing again this this childish thinking,
(05:51):
and they were talking about um an arts program in
which the kids were making different kinds of clay models,
and one of the kids came up with Bacon Boy
and it is so I've got to throw up an
image of this. It is awesome, you know, sort of
a superpower figure. And again, these are not things that
we as adults go around thinking like I'm going to
(06:11):
sculpture Bacon Boy today and really start to think about
this mythology of this character. But that's what kids do.
And um, if you have ever taken a walk with
a toddler anywhere from two to when they started to
get preschool four five years of age, you know there
is no linear path. That this is going to take
a long time because everything is going to be picked
(06:34):
up and inspected and stories where will begin to just
organically arise from their experience with with their environments. And
we have talked about this before, but I thought it
would be good for us to mention that this is
this idea that when we are adults, we have a
focus that is that's pretty laser focused, this flashlight focus
(06:57):
that kids starting as infants grow into that flashlight focus,
but they begin at the lantern of light experience where everything,
uh is it has light cast upon it and they're
considering everything in their world. Yeah. Like, one thing that
comes to mind when I think about this is the
the Alan Rogue Grulay novel Jealousy, where the entire book
(07:21):
and this is not going to really sell it well
from most people, but the entire book is this guy
staring at a wall or occasionally staring at its has
banana plantation and trying to figure out whether his wife
is having an affair with another band of plantation owner.
And so a lot of it is him staring at
at a sneer on the wall where he killed a
centipede and just obsessing and obsessing, obsessing. It's that laser focus,
(07:44):
you know, and that he's getting nothing done the whole
novel because he's just obsessed with one thing. And I
feel in his adults that we often do that it's
not an actual smash centipede on the wall. Then it's
something like, you know, some you know guy starts losing
his hair, and then that's the thing they come back
to over and over again. Oh my goodness, what's happening
to me? Am I I'm getting older. I'm dying. You know,
we end up obsessing over something, or we get obsessed
(08:05):
with with one particular material thing or another, or or
we attached our ego to a sports team or something.
Whereas how many how many child children do you know
who are rabid sports fanatics, who are rabbid fans of
a particular team, you know, how many reliduced fundamentalist children
do you know? How many neurotic children do you know
where they're obsessed with? Uh, I don't know if their
(08:27):
weight gain. Yeah, that's just not really something that you
you commonly see until they get a little bit older, right,
they get into grade school. The thing about this, and
psychologist Alison Gothnick has talked about this, is that when
you're an adult, you have, uh, you know, certain neural
connections that have been pruned away because you don't use
them anymore. And so if you are that character in
(08:49):
the book who is staring at the wall, well, you're
gonna be squirting a lot of neuro transmitters on that
part of the brain to really keep it activated in focus.
But if you are an infant, your entire brain is
just steeped in neuro transmitters. It's marinating in it. And
this is what she says results in this information rich world,
(09:10):
this lantern vision, trying to take every single thing in. Yeah.
She mentions in her in her writing that in the
past it's been difficult for us to try and study
exactly what's going on in the in the minds of
young children, and certainly in the minds of of unlanguaged
children and infants, and and even if you can get
them to talk, it's going to be a stream of
consciousness mumbo jumbo about I think her example is birthday
(09:31):
parties and horses, obviously for for girls and for for
little boys. Birthday parties. Uh, fire trucks and boogers. You know,
it's I mean to go across gender, across gender, fire trucks,
but yeah, um so. So I found that interesting because
a lot of her studies is about getting beyond that
and really finding ways to not only look at what
(09:54):
children and infants are saying, well certainly not infants so much,
but what beyond what children are say, and also look
at their actions and how they're interacting with the world
around them. And what she gets out a lot is
the idea of this plasticity, which we've talked about before,
the ability of our mind to change, the ability of
ourselves to to to roll with the punches, because when
you were a a zero to three year old, you
(10:17):
have to really be able to roll with the punches.
And so even in even in a very comfy environment,
if you grow up in a very civilized environment, a
very safe environment, there's still a lot of a lot
of trauma around you when you're that alert to the world,
and certainly in in less advantageous environments, you've gotta be
really hardy to survive. Yeah, she alson. Gopnik actually has
(10:40):
this great quote about what it's like to be an infant,
and she said, and this is the kind of more
romanticized version of it, as opposed to just you know,
having to deal with all the noise and the different stimuli.
She says, it's like being in love and perish for
the first time after you've had three double espressos. Yeah,
so there is this idea you are being bombarded by
all the different elements out there. She also mentions the
(11:02):
study of Eastern European orphans who were adopted by parents
in the UK and about how in in in most
of the case it's not all but in most of
those cases, and these were kids that were they're growing
up in like extreme um situations of you know, they're
just not exposed to enough sensory information, enough personal interaction.
(11:22):
They're starved for all of this. But they're so but
the child is so resilient zero to three that most
of them were able to just really bounce back without
any kind of significant problems. And she says that this
is because of this different consciousness that kids have that
they grow out of again into the more flashlight focus.
(11:43):
But she's saying, and this is this is kind of
looping it back on how you can try to retain
a bit of this for yourself as as an adult.
She's saying that the creative people are able to hold
onto this different consciousness to be able to inhabit this
mind space where you can transfer your consciousness from flashlight
(12:03):
to lantern and began to take more things in all
while being a reasonably um responsible adult. Of course, one
thing that comes to mind here, and so we're comparing
adult artists with young children, one thing that instantly comes
to mind here is children running around in the yard naked,
and then artists inevitably running around in the yard naked,
(12:25):
or like our friend Picasso here in the diaper um
because it does kind of his head where it does
look like a diaper, Because art, like childhood, is kind
of a judgment free zone. And as it turns out,
that plays into this mind of a child, this creativity
as well. Yeah, and in fact, in kids and teenagers
frontal lobes the seat of judgment. Right, these are the
last pieces to be fully connected to the brain, okay,
(12:48):
or be fully connected to parts of the brain that
deal with judgment, inhibition, self awareness, cause and effect, acknowledgement, um,
all the things that are sort of the bane of
our existence with with teenagers that we normally look at us. Ah, man,
they just they're crazy there, you know, look at their hair.
What are they think? Yeah, yeah, they're just there's not
a lick of sense. And that kid um, that actually
(13:09):
can be a real boon two kids because they lack
again this idea of this inner judgmental voice that can
sometimes stop us in our tracks when we're trying to
do something novel um. So again, trying to silence that
part of the brain is really important. And we have
talked about this before, but surgeon and jazz musician Charles
(13:30):
Limb wanted to look at this a little more carefully
to say, how how are musicians so adept at just
getting in there and improvising? What makes them able to
do that? And it turns out that musicians are really
good at turning off the part of the brain again
this uh dorsal lateral prefrontal and lateral orbital regions, dimming
(13:54):
that and instead bringing online the medial prefrontal cortex, which
else them to express themselves better. And so the frontal
lobes they dim a bit because they're like, yeah, you
know what, I don't need you right now. I really
need to kind of flex this part of my muscle.
So he saw that in all these m r I
scans of these musicians, which really pointed to this idea
(14:15):
that certain things are play. In fact, neuroscientists, Rex Jung
also talks about how highly creative people usually have less
white matter integrity and less brain tissue in the frontal lobes. Okay,
that doesn't mean that they're you know, less intelligent or
that they lack something. It just means that the frontal
lobes again the seat of judgment, Uh, it's not nearly
(14:37):
as taxed with neural connections, these glial cells, white cells,
and therefore those people are a bit more unencumbered when
it comes to creating something new. All right, we are
going to take a quick break, but when we come back,
we are going to talk about the importance of play
in this idea of uncertainty in our lives. All right,
(15:04):
we're back. We're discussing again the minds of children. Uh,
this old idea that children see the world as it
really is, or they see it in in a unique way.
And if we as adults can simply recapture some of
that childhood essence, and I mean not in a drain
essence from a child's skull in a like a skexy way,
but in an actual let us let's change the way
(15:25):
we perceive the world and how we interact with it too,
to improve our creative output. And and when I say creative,
I'm not just talking about finger painting on the wall.
But as we'll discussed here, actual scientific achievement as well
falls under this category. Yeah, really engaging critical thinking skill.
According to neuroscientists, Bolatto and he has that great Ted
(15:48):
dot com talk, He says that uncertainty for adults is
really problematic. And this is particularly true in the context
of evolution, where uncertainty, you know, not knowing if there's
a saber tooth tiger in the weeds over there or
if it's just the wind rustling through through the leaves,
could result in death. For us, we need to be
(16:08):
certain about certain elements of life Yeah. That's the thing
about uncertainty is that, for the most part, most of
us tend to want to get away from uncertainty because
uncertainty brings potential disaster. Uncertainty as you're walking down the
street means maybe I'll get run over by a car,
maybe I'll get mugged. Maybe again the saber through tiger
will jump out at me. But it's out of that
(16:30):
uncertainty that so many amazing creative ideas arise. And the
way that we combat uncertainty, of course, is to sort
of apply a script to the world, to cling to
certain world views that that bring order out of chaos.
So look at any particular world view and it may
and generally it's about positioning yourself, your group at the center,
(16:53):
creating a barrier between this group and outside groups. I
we think like this, They think like that. These are
the rules of the environment in which I live. These
are the people who don't abide by those rules. These
are the rules that apply to me. We we steadily organized,
We build a little fort of ideas in which we
feel safe against the chaos of the world. And that's
(17:16):
that's certainty that we're talking about. That really helps us
to predict how things will come out. But he's saying
that for children, uncertainty is a game, and it is
a necessary game because if you look at animals and humans,
all species play in some way. And we've talked about
this before, um in terms of the amount of time
it takes for a creature to mature. We've talked about
(17:37):
the New Caledonian crow, which has a relatively long childhood
as as a bird, and then we've talked about the say,
just you know, your run of the Milk chicken, and
the consensus there was that the New Caldonian crow really
needed that time to mature because it's pretty sophisticated in
terms of its tool use and where's the chicken so
(18:00):
much doesn't need a lot of time to play to
to have a long childhood. And as we said, one
ended up in in a pot. The other one, the
New Caldonian Crow, ended up on the cover of Nature
magazine because it does have these very sophisticated tool using abilities.
So when you look at children and you get uncertainty,
(18:21):
it really is necessary for kids to throw away the
rules and to begin to like a scientist, approach their
environments and play with that environment. Yeah, it's easy to
look at children and play and and just sort of
discredited to say, oh, that's just children wasting time instead
of doing chores, which they should be doing, or to
look at it in terms of all right, well that's
(18:41):
a that's a boy and he's playing with tools and
beating stuff with hammers. He's just he's just kind of
practicing for his life. Or oh, there's a little girl
and she's playing with the baby doll. Well, that's just
her rehearsing. That's a little there's a kitten fighting another kitten.
They're just rehearsing for their their lives. It's aggressive hunters,
but there's a lot more going on speci Typically, as
a bow Latto points out, play be it whatever the
(19:05):
kittens doing, what the child is doing, or what an
adult artists or scientists is engaging in, boils down to
five different things. First, celebrating uncertainty. It's not you're not
entering the environment and saying saying, oh, there there might
be something I'm unsure of outside of this fort of ideas.
It's about venturing outside of that fort of ideas and
(19:26):
seeing the world anew uh. It's about being acceptable to change,
engaging in this in this world, beyond the fortress of ideas,
and realizing that what you see may change you, It
may change how you assemble your fortress of ideas when
you return to it. You have to be open to possibility,
(19:47):
open to the possibility that you're gonna change, open to
the possibility that your preconceived notions are going to fail.
You need to be cooperative, certainly if you're venturing outside
of that fortress of ideas with other individuals, and finally,
intrinsically motivated, you're doing it because you want to You
want to see beyond this fortress of ideas that you've
used to understand the world previously. Now, as um, you
(20:09):
have probably witnessed before with kids that the rules can
change pretty quick, right, They can throw them out or
sometimes just completely change them. One second the floor's lava,
then it's then it's a never ending pit, and then
it's okay to walk on, which is great, right because
it kind of gets very good narrative consistency. No, nobody
gives them a certain amount of flexibility. And you have
(20:30):
mentioned like giving a girl a baby doll and giving
a boy tools. Um, really, you give a kid a
stick and they are going to turn it into something
in which Tory, Yeah, to interrogate the world around them.
And I see this again and again with my daughter
making Pulley systems out of you know, um buckets, because
(20:51):
the buckets are full of Pixie destin she needs to
skewer some pirate or something. Um. But I think it's
really interestinging that Lotto looked at this play, this uncertainty,
and then he sought out a group of children ages
eight to ten because he wanted to know could they
approach an experiment or could they create their own experiment?
(21:14):
And could we actually get something on the other end
that we could use. Because again he's saying that those
those five circumstances that you talked about, this openness, this
um intrinsically motivated cooperation, and so on and so forth.
He's saying that this is really the play of a scientist.
This is what scientists do. Yeah. I mean again, it's
easier to make the initial comparison to creative work because
(21:35):
like I think, if like, if I'm writing a story
or something, I'm entering it with an uncertain mind. I
don't know where characters are gonna end up, or what's
going to happen in maybe one rule or two that
you're trying to stay too, and those rules may they
break by the time I finish it. I mean, I
can't think of the number of times that I've I've
started writing a story and when I end up getting
is entirely different because I've because you've got to be
open to it to change. And then, certainly in science,
(21:57):
when you start looking at these these five things from
a scientist per spective, imagine if a scientist goes out
to figure out why something in the world works the
way it does, or something in the outer cosmos works
the way it does. You're going to go in there
with some preconceived notions, but you have to be able
to to dispel them and ignore them. It need be
for commisically and you have to celebrate uncertainty, it be
acceptable to change, open to possibility, cooperative, and intrinsically motivated. Well,
(22:21):
a lot of wanted to see if these codes could
see themselves differently through the process of being a scientist,
because again, when you say let's let's do some science,
that's pretty weighted because people approach it and thinking that
science is something that is separate from them as opposed
to well, it's actually science really plays to our strengths
as human beings. Yeah, this this comes into sort of
the puff the magic dragon thing again. There's kind of
(22:43):
this false idea that when you're a child, it's all
interacting with imaginary creatures and engaging in this creativity. But
then you learn to be a grown up and you
learn science, and then you put all that crap behind you.
But as as we're discussing here, being a scientist is
as much about embracing that spirit of childhood as it
is about growing up and becoming more mature. So a
(23:05):
lot of worked with twenty five children in conjunction with
their headmaster, again ages eight to ten, and they studied
black wotton bees to see if these bees, again this
is something that kids came up with, could solve problems
in a similar way that humans do. And the kids
asked the questions and they actually devised the experiments. Um.
(23:27):
According to Biology Letters, which published the paper, the children's
findings show that bees are able to alter their foraging
behavior based on previously learned colors and pattern cues and
a complex scene consisting of a local pattern within a
larger global pattern. This is pretty sophisticated for an insect
um and then in Biology letters that says, as there
(23:49):
has been little testing of bees learning color patterns at
small and large scales, the results contribute considerably to our
understanding of insect behavior. The kids managed to not only
published a paper, which we'll talk a little bit more
about how they did that, but to to get a
novel understanding of an insect. And what I love about
(24:11):
this is that the paper actually begins with once upon
a Time, which largely written in kids speak, but the
methodology that they came up with, the observations, the hypothesis,
all of this is so solid that they, you know,
the people who actually ended up reviewing this and writing
a commentary on this could not deny that they had
(24:33):
found something that was very valuable. Well, it reminds me
of your daughter's interaction with the trial bide and how
she granted she gave it, that she give it a name,
called it Gonk, but then also created the story about
how it was going down to the water and eating plants,
if I remember correctly. So it wasn't living in a
fairy castle or anything. It was. She wasn't creating a
a plausible story for that creature based on her knowledge
(24:56):
of the world well and more importantly of her observations
of the world, then she had a context for it,
and she of course subscribed emotions to it and all
this sort of stuff. But yeah, I mean, do you
just give kids a couple of things and they will
run with us? And UH and began to see the
logic inherent in there. I wanted to also point out
that the paper that they came up with actually had
(25:17):
hand drawn figures and tables in it as well. And
if you check out that TED talk um by a
lot of you'll also see a presentation by Amy O'Toole, who,
at the time of a twelve year old student who
helped run one of the science experiments that it was
inspired by Bolto science approach and UH. At the age
of ten, she became one of the youngest people ever
to publish a peer of viewed science paper, and she
(25:39):
was also at the time the youngest person to give
a TED talk or to help give a TED talk.
Since you get the second half, but that was really
motivating as well, because then it comes down to the
idea that when you got these kids in the room,
you got him thinking about science they were already asking
questions that were significant to science. Yeah, I mean this
is pretty life changing for a lot of those kids,
because again a lot of wanted to see how they
would see themselves after going through a scientific process and
(26:05):
you know, help them to gell this idea that science
is again a part of them and not apart, not
some sort of It's not something you learn, it's something
you are from the earliest. If anything is we grow
older more many of us we forget science rather than
have to learn it. Well, it just seems like something
that we look through the window at, right. But Loto
said this, and I thought it was really interesting. He said,
(26:27):
the point is what science does for us. We normally
walk through life responding, but if we ever want to
do anything different, we have to step into uncertainty. That's
what science offers us. It offers the possibility to step
into uncertainty through the process of play. Yeah. It reminds
me of a quote badly enough by Timothy Leary who
talked about before he gets his childlike was childlike. Yeah,
(26:48):
I mean he was, you know, he was very free
thinking dude. Um for some of the other faults aside,
but he has this great quote where he says to
think for yourself, you must question an authority and learn
how to put yourself in a state of vulnerable, open mindedness,
chaotic confused vulnerability to inform yourself tune in. Yeah, and
(27:09):
of course the thing is, you know, you need to
remind mrr Larry that ultimately don't need any pharmaceutical help
to achieve that. In I mean, all you have to
do is either be a child or try and think
like a child, and you can achieve that that level
of of chaotic, confused vulnerability. Well, here's the thing about
Larry is that he was a trained scientist, and so
(27:29):
he had a background in the best ways to go
about thinking critically but also thinking in a way that
could really open up the mind. And when I think
about ways in which you can look at probabilities and
try to predict the future in a new way or
an interesting way, I think about Bayesian modeling. Yes. Now,
this is UH something that is named after the Reverend
(27:50):
Reverend Thomas Bayes, an eighteenth century mathematician, and according to
Alice and Gopnik, studies show that kids at least unconsciously
are Asian UH masters themselves. Now here's the thing about
Beiesian logic is you can really get into the weeds
trying to understand what it is. Essentially, Baisian probability theory
(28:10):
is a branch of mathematical theory that allows one to
model uncertainty about the world and about the outcomes of
various aspects of that world by combining common sense knowledge
with observational evidence. Okay, it sounds very mechanical and straightforar
this because it is. It's actually figures into some of
our AI constructs that we're working on today. It's a
(28:32):
central part of trying to figure out how an intelligent
creature thinks. Yeah, it was actually going to mention that
Bayesian modeling actually came online in I think the early nineties,
around the same time that psychologists were beginning to look
at kids and wondering if this Baisian modeling was inherent
to them. And it's funny because AI, artificial intelligence and
(28:55):
kids really go hand in hand because people who are
interested in AI are interested in looking at kids as
the root model. In other words, if we're going to
build a computer that can think like us, act like us,
make decisions, then we want it to be based on
this root material. A k a. Kids. And you know,
as we're moving forward to the future, we we inevitably
(29:17):
come back to this idea of of robots solving problems,
computers solving problems. We want to know the weather's doing
in ten days through a computer model at it, right,
And it's just gonna become more and more like that
as we move forward. So it's it's fascinating to think
that the ais that we're building today to solve the
problems of tomorrow, children are already born with that, with
(29:38):
those mechanics in their mind. If they get older and
they become adults, it becomes clouded. So in a way,
adult humans are having to build robots that think like
children so they can solve the problems that they no
longer can. And one of the reasons that can because
of preconceived notions. And these again, these these fortresses of
ideas that we build because whether you're looking at questions
(29:59):
of I want who's gonna win the election, I wonder,
I wonder what choice I should make in my life
regarding my employment, uh, any number of questions that may
come up. We're we're handicapped against applying the Asian logic
to them because we have these these world views in place,
these preconceived notions, this fortress of ideas that we have
(30:20):
to somehow navigate, and it could because of all those
preconceived notions, they end up flowing the data. Yeah, it's true.
And um, you know, kids can really be better problem
solvers when it comes to Bayesian logic, because, as you say,
there are certain things that as we get older, we
have these priors, they get stronger and stronger, and they
actually need to in some ways to help us survive
(30:42):
our experience in the world. But we're already relying on
that too heavily and less on new data, right, so
we rely more on our past experiences, and these strong
priors are really actually very comforting to us. But Bayesian
inference inference, excuse me, it considers both uh, new evidence
and prior probability of hypotheses, and this gives Baisian learning
(31:06):
a character characteristic combination of stability and flexibility. So here's
the key to it really working really well in science
and with kids. In science. Uh, if if you have
a really crappy hypothesis, you're gonna throw it out. Kids
are going to do the same thing. They don't have
emotional investments or really strong priors. So this allows them
(31:29):
to go through the information much better. Okay, so how
do we know that kids are better at some reasoning
when it comes to to be Asian modeling, um than adults?
Is it because they say the darniest things? It is
Alson Gopnik again, who is pretty much the centerpiece for
this podcast. She used something called a Blikic detector. This
(31:52):
is a machine that lights up and plays music when
certain objects which are controlled by the experiment or are
placed on top of it, plays like a cube onto
this little platform and lights up and it starts playing music. Right,
you might have different shapes, you might have a star
or a cube, or different colors, and so the idea
is that you begin to understand the relationship of what
makes the machine work. Well, what she did is she
(32:14):
asked both adults and children separately in separate experiments, to
try to figure out these objects and how they would
make the machine work. I mean, essentially, she was saying,
go make this machine work. Well, the kids were a
lot better at it, because what what the adults did
is they observed what the experiment or did with the
blocks to make the machine work and there's a really
(32:34):
strong priors, and so they were holding onto the this
idea of what happened in the past, whereas the kids
were able to take every single angle of the blocks
the colors put together, you know, various points of data
to figure out how to best make this blicket machine work.
Another example of this is give an iPad to a
(32:55):
child and your grandparents and see who figures it out
for Yeah, and in some not to say that's a
complete divide, because you're gonna have some older people that
really dive with new technology and again are really able
to pass that booket test. Like my wife's grandmother in
her nineties use the Kindle all the time, which is awesome.
(33:15):
Do you think kindles in the beginning like an early
early adopter of that technology. Well, and see there's again
there's this idea of holding on see this bit of
your your childhood thinking or your childness, or this openness
to new ideas and experiences and not saying up a
machine for a bug, give me my old books, Just
give me my paper, my paper, I just move with
my finger. Yeah, yeah, you can do the same thing
(33:37):
with kindle. Um, all right, So there's this idea. Another
pcassa quote that everything you can imagine is real. Now
I would say that I would add this, you just
have to make it fit into that bay Asian model.
So anything you dream of could be real as long
as you can make it fit into the constructs of
(33:57):
our physical world. And that's where Okem's razor comes in. Yeah,
all comes razor. Is this idea that basically the simplest
answer is the one that is probably the most likely answer. Yeah,
Akams raisers. It's interesting because the term first appears around
eighteen fifty, two centuries after the death of the guy's
named after, who was a fourteenth century Franciscan friar by
(34:20):
the name of William of Oakum. Uh. Really fascinating dude. Um.
If you've ever read the Name of the rose By
and Burn of Eco, the main character in that, who's
a Sherlock Holmes styled monk named William of Baskerville. He's
always talking about his friendship and camaraderie with Oakum's way
of thinking, because despite being a Franciscan friar, he was
you could you imagine, all right, what he's gonna be
(34:40):
a really religious dude. He's gonna see the world through
religious goggles and to a certain extent you, as we've
discussed in a recent podcast on witchcraft, that's gonna be
a part of the way you see the world. You
just can't help with us the world you're born into.
But Oakum was a realist, and what we call a nominalist.
Nominalism is the theory that there are no universal soul,
(35:01):
essences and reality. He argued that only individuals exist rather
than super individual universals, essences, and forms. So to break
that down in a really sustinct way, I turned to
Catholic Encyclopedia of all places. They have a really nice
paragraph and we're just gonna read this. They say, exaggerated
realism invents a world of reality, corresponding exactly to the
(35:22):
attributes of the world of thought. Nominalism, on the contrary,
models the concept on the external object, which it holds
to be individual in particular. So it comes down to
how are you gonna understand the world around you? Are
you gonna start with the ideas about the world and
work down to the world itself, or do you start
with the world and work out from there? And so
(35:44):
that's that's a large part of Lum's Razer. Right there
is is looking at this idea, this possible theory for
what's happening, and then asking yourself which hypothesis conforms to
the world that we observe and not the world that
we think exists. This is the basis of the scientific method,
and so when we talk about science and scientists and play,
(36:05):
this is really essentially we're talking about because in a way,
this is a bit of a thought experiment, although with
the thought experiment you're just trying to kind of throw
everything out there. You're not really looking for for something
that's going to stick. But it's the same idea that
you you try to figure out every hypothesis you can
and then you call out the ones that don't make
(36:26):
any sense or at least probable. Um. Now, this is
really an awesome game to play with kids because it
fuels their imagination, but it also gives them the tools
to sort through all the data that they have and
find a line of logic. And it's a particularly a
nice game to play when they're at that age where
(36:46):
they're asking a bazilion questions about how the world works. Right, Yeah,
like why is that? Why is the sky blue? Well,
because it has to do with reflectional well, why is
that the case? Then? Why is that the case? And
and you can either give up like the lady I
observed at a zoo once when the child asked, Mommy,
why does the commode dragon? Why does he look like that?
And she just responded because that's just the way God
(37:06):
made him, honey. Right there, there's a complete non answer,
and not engaging with the child's curiosity about the world
and instead saying, here's a big wall of the fortress
of ideas, let me erect that in in the way
of the horizon. Say I'm going to say that that
parent probably had low blood sugar. So before you engage
in augms phraser, make sure you eat something because you're
(37:27):
gonna need that energy because kids will ask a million questions,
as you say, and it can get a little bit like, oh,
you know, like after fifteen million questions. But you know, obviously,
my daughter asked me a ton of questions every day
about everything, and um, one of the things that I
noticed was she's starting to enter into that territory where
the unknown is frightening her. So she's really looking for answers,
(37:51):
she's looking for comfort, Like the skeleton thing you mentioned. Yeah,
she's she's very frightened of skeletons, even though we've talked
about how they're inside our bodies. Um, you know, they
help us to walk, so on and so forth, a
very practical things. She said, I don't care. I just
don't want to see them outside of the skin, which
is a reasonable request. What are you know what you're
gonna say to that. Um, But one of the things
that drives her nuts, or has in the past, is
(38:13):
that we'll she'll hear things on the roof of our house. Well,
I know it's magnolia pods. There's just falling and they're
huge and they're I mean, they do sound like someone's
on the roof. Yeah, oh yeah, the squirrels. It's a
racetrack actually on our house, particularly this time of year
in the fall. But um, so if I hear allowed that,
what I've done in the past is I've said, okay,
(38:34):
you know, let's look out the window and see what's
on the ground, and they will observe that there are
magnolia pods all over the yard. So then I can
ask my daughter, and I've done this before, which is, hey, okay,
what else do you think could be causing that noise.
So now we're entering into this idea of blackham threachers,
where we're going to gather as many hypotheses as possible,
and so she's come up with before you know, again,
(38:57):
the skeleton is a skeleton. It's trying to come down
the gym name get me um Or she has observed
that there's construction in our neighborhood, and she said that
it's a construction crane, and she thinks that it just
came down the street and crashed into our roof. Okay,
but she's already going from from least believable hypothesis to
a slightly more believable hypothesis. Yeah, because actually, if you
(39:17):
count the number of assumptions for all of the hypotheses,
you will see that the Magnoli pod is one assumption
it fell from the tree in Atlanta on the ground.
But a skeleton, well, that is requires the assumption that
even though it's dead, it doesn't have any flesh around
it somehow alive, it somehow has a functioning brain. Those
three it can scale a roof and show me down
(39:39):
a chimney that's for maybe even five um and then
it has some business with us. It's got some some
reason for coming down the chimney and talking to us
about that too. Is it makes me think, well, to
make the skeleton down the chimney idea makes sense. You
really need a working um understanding of necromancy, which is
just say, you need a fortress of ideas that has
(40:01):
been carefully constructed by adults to make the unlikely seem plausible. Yeah,
you need so, I say five assumptions there, but really,
if you're going to detail, there's probably about a hundred
different assumptions there. So then you get to the crane
and now you have the crane down the street scenario
that it can operate on its own. That's an assumption
that it has somehow managed to make its way through
(40:22):
four houses and and and and still has momentum that
it has bumped into the roof, and that somehow, even
though it's bunched in the roof, there's no damage done
to our house. There's four assumptions. You're right, there's getting better. Yeah,
because if if the if the walking skeleton necromance, the
idea of that is like it's like a cathedral of
ideas with flying buttresses and stuff. Whereas the claim moving
(40:45):
on its own. That's more like a decent um cabin
in the woods, kind of fortress of ideas, modestly constructed
but still constructed. Nice. Nice. I like that, Yes, flying
buttresses as opposed to like a modest cabin. So you
present all those which I've done with my daughter before,
and she'll just laugh and say the magnolia pod because
then she's starting to understand that these are outrageous things.
(41:08):
But we've been able to talk about really cool different
ways that the world might work, which is the imagination
in the creativity part. But now she has something to
hold onto that is concrete, that is logical, that makes sense. Yeah,
so she can imagine the flying buttresses and the crazy
cathedral of ideas. I mean, everyone wants to be able
to imagine something that that rich and and and just engaging,
(41:30):
you know, kind of like uh, like say Dante's Inferno.
I love that that's the cathedral ideas that there ever
was one. But when I actually think about how the
world works, I choose to go with a far more
modest construction of ideas um and certainly at times of
the less the less building there on the horizon, the better,
And you can actually see the world as it perhaps
(41:50):
really is now. Gothnick says that these cathedral of ideas,
this is really the evolutionary juice of our species. We
have to have this imagine a because she said, you know,
think about every single thing around you right now. Think
about this microphone in front of us. This was once
an idea in someone's head and it was part of
their imagination, and they just used their available knowledge to
(42:15):
create this thing. Uh, you know, given a couple of
constructs of what is possible what is not possible. So
you know, she's saying that if you are um, a
human fifty thou years ago, this is incredibly important as well,
because you're trying to imagine or predict really what the
year is going to look for you. So you start
to really pay attention to seasons when some animals might
(42:37):
be migrating, right, and you start to sort of imagine
yourself participating in this future self or this future part
of yourself. So again, just all of this is ah
I think, an evolutionary boon to us, this stability to imagine, create,
play and essentially become scientists. All right, Well, on that note,
(42:58):
let's call over the robot and get some listener mail here.
The first one comes from a listener by the name
of h. A. H A writes in and says, greetings
Robert and Julie, regarding your mention of Bloody Mary in
your recent podcast Light as a Feather Stiff as a Board.
I've always, personally, i albeit humorously, believed that the Bloody
(43:19):
Mary myth that she would appear of her name was
spoken a certain number of times in front of a
mirror was real, but that everybody that's tried it so
far had just gotten the number of times one has
to say her name to invoke her wrong. I believe
the correct number is three hundred and thirty three. No,
I've never tried it anyway. Thank you for the entertaining
and informative podcast. We'll see there's a there's an interesting,
(43:40):
uh cathedral of ideas that only not a cathedral. But
let's say there's a modest cabin of ideas, and our
listener here built a little extension. Well, yeah, what is
half of if you double three three, what is that? Well,
that would be six D sixty six. Yeah, so curious
to know why it would be half of the devil's numbers,
and that their math is wrong, that the actually needs
(44:02):
to be doubled up or I don't know, maybe maybe
Bloody Mary is going to match that number on the
other side and then there then becomes complete. Well, it
reminds me of pretty much every attempt to predict to
the end of the world, be at the rapture or
something else. Inevitably that comes a point where that where
someone says, well, the math was wrong. We need to
do the math a little more correctly to figure out
(44:23):
exactly when the world's gonna end. So I'm sure if
if someone were to count up to thirty three, uh,
there would be a need to revise our predictions speaking
of when is the next end of the world, Oh
like now or something that the mind calendar. When what's
happening right now outside the door? Oh yeah, that's what
that was. I just thought there was a lot of traffic.
We also heard from a listener by the name of Kristen.
(44:43):
Kristen writes and says, hey, guys, in the Maps episodes,
you talked about how humans are just wired to respond
to vertical and horizontal and diagonals and mess things up.
It triggered a memory for me three summers ago, I
visited my friend in Colorado and we went to an
old West type town. They had a mystery house there.
You went inside. It started out with the hallway floor
(45:04):
slanted at a slight angle, which was fine, and then
it opened up into this huge room and the floor
there was at a forty five degree angle. I guess
my brain totally shut down because as soon as I
stepped into the room, I just fell down into the
wall and couldn't move or stop laughing. My friend's dad
had to help me out because I could not move.
L o l uh. It's my favorite memory. Thanks for
(45:24):
the great podcast. That's because that draws us right into
the episode we did on Haunted Houses to how if
you screwed around with the shape of rooms and engage
more of that diagonal construction, then it throws us off.
It throws throws off our ability to predict what's going
to happen, our understanding where we are in a space.
That's right because we have many more neurons that are
dedicated to the X Y access than the diagonal access.
(45:47):
So it makes sense that our brain would like this
very clean lines, and of course haunted houses like to
play with that idea, so that's fascinating. Thanks for writing
into us, Kristen and h A. We always love to
hear from our our listeners and if you have anything
to add on this podcast, let us know. And we
know we have a lot of parents out there as
their listeners, and then many more who have children in
(46:10):
their lives, and many more still who are very talented artists,
uh and creative people. We'd love to hear from you
guys as well. How do how does the child inside
you come out when you engage and creative act? Send
us an example of what you do to We'd love
to share it on Facebook and let us know how
the young lauarvel human in your life, How they seem
(46:31):
to see the world around you, What kind of questions
are they asking, What unique inside are they bringing the
table when they construct their own fortress of ideas. You
can find us on Facebook, you can find us on tumbler.
We are stuff to blow your mind on both of
those and on Twitter we go by the handle blow
the Mind and you can always drop us a line
at Blow the Mind at discovery dot com. For more
(46:58):
on this and thousands of other topics, visit how Stuff
Works dot com.