Episode Transcript
Available transcripts are automatically generated. Complete accuracy is not guaranteed.
Speaker 1 (00:00):
And I think having a relatively richer understanding of emotions,
how they work, feelings, moral emotions, moral psychology has helped
in my conversations with kids and with the staff who
work with them, teachers, counselors, clinicians, doctors.
Speaker 2 (00:17):
So it's informed how I speak to the individual I
work with, which includes the kids.
Speaker 1 (00:24):
In one of the podcasts I was on, it was
kind of reframed as second chances.
Speaker 2 (00:29):
All the kids I work with are second chance kids.
Speaker 3 (00:32):
I love this. Today it's my great pleasure to introduce
Mark Hauser to the podcast. Mark is a researcher, educator, consultant,
former Harvard professor, and author of the recent book Vulnerable Minds,
The Harm of Childhood Trauma and the Hope of Resilience.
Doctor Hauser's scientific research includes over three hundred published papers
(00:54):
and seven books focusing on how the brain evolves, develops,
and is altered by damage and neurodevelopment disorder. In this
rich and important discussion, we talked about adverse child experiences
or aces, and why it's important to distinguish them from trauma.
We also discussed Mark's new framework for thinking about the
dimensions of childhood adversity and the challenges and promises of
implementing the kind of approach he advocates for. This was
(01:16):
an ultimately uplifting and hopeful discussion and I'm really glad
to see the work Mark is doing to help all
kinds of vulnerable minds. So, without further ado, I bring
you Mark Hauser. Mark Hauser, Wow, welcome to the Psychology Podcast.
Speaker 2 (01:30):
Lovely to be here.
Speaker 3 (01:31):
Oh, it's so good to see you, Sane. What incredible
new book that you've written here that covers a lot
of themes that are very prominent in our culture today,
such as adversity, challenge, trauma, and then you focus on
you know where it sometimes matters the most in our childhood, right, yes, yes,
(01:53):
So how many years now have you been working on
the issue of childhood adversity?
Speaker 1 (02:00):
So it kind of depends on how you answer the
question of work on because in many ways my kind
of research history has been heavily influenced by developmental ideas,
you know, going back to collaborations with Nolam Chomsky and
Steve Pinker, which are of course developmental issues, but evolutionary
issues too. But in terms of the practical side of it,
(02:22):
which is really kind of the new piece about fourteen
years now working in schools, and of course not all
of it has been really on children with trauma histories,
but children with different kinds of disabilities.
Speaker 3 (02:34):
Well that's right, Maale. You know, yes, very nice in neurodiversity. Yes,
and to what extent do you think neurodiversity? I wouldn't
expect to start off with this question, but yeah, you
have me thinking, now, can neudiversity be an outcome of adversity? So?
I think so.
Speaker 1 (02:51):
I think in a sense that is often used today,
the neurodiversity kind of movement or idea really has to
do with how we think about disability in some ways.
I think that's how I at least would frame it, and
that we have a diversity of options that our genomes
in some ways create, affected by heather.
Speaker 2 (03:12):
Interact with the environment.
Speaker 1 (03:14):
So traumatic experiences can certainly shape how we respond. And
you know, soon we'll get into these ideas. But sometimes
the traumatic experience can lead to what the psychologist Bruce
Ellis talks about as hidden talents that which I really
like as well, and sometimes, of course it can really
undermine real functioning.
Speaker 3 (03:34):
Sure, yeah, I'm glad that you brought the Bruce Ellis
work on that because he actually draws a little bit
on my advisor's work, Robert Sternberg. Yes, I'm talking about
how some of these kids can develop practical intelligence. Yes,
they can go underlooked in our schools. So I'm sure
you've seen that.
Speaker 1 (03:49):
Too, absolutely, And I think what's come out of that
work as a sort of a side effect, in some sense,
there's been.
Speaker 2 (03:55):
Some beautiful work on.
Speaker 1 (03:58):
Autism where a lot of the cognitive biases that non
autistic individuals succumb to autistic individuals do not, and.
Speaker 2 (04:09):
That paints is very interesting.
Speaker 1 (04:10):
For example, some of the rational biases, like the famous
bat and ball experiment of you know, a dollar and
ten cents, you get much fewer individuals.
Speaker 2 (04:19):
Or autistic you know falling.
Speaker 1 (04:26):
And that, I mean, that's you know, that's fascinating. It
paints an interesting picture about just, you know, diversity of
ways of viewing the world.
Speaker 3 (04:33):
Yeah. I often think I might be a bit on
the spectrum. I can't stand pretense. I'm allergic to pretense,
you know, but some people seem to love the pretense.
It seems like most of the world. When scroll through Instagram,
I feel like I'm scrolling through a screen of pretense.
What's going on? Why is that so? Why is pretense
so awarring to humans?
Speaker 1 (04:52):
I don't know the seductive nature of that, And I
don't know if it's because the social media world has
just sucked people into that universe so much. I think
it's interesting that you say that you kind of have
an allergy to it. You know, it's interesting in the
schools I work in, where there are some schools that
have a very high proportion of individuals on the spectrum,
(05:13):
it's funny. You'll you'll see these kinds of kids kind
of posing and doing all these things.
Speaker 2 (05:18):
And the autistic kids are just like, look, let's get
out of here. Don't want anything to do with it.
Speaker 3 (05:23):
We're not big in a small talk. No small talk. Well,
let's talk about aces because it's a it's a controversial
topic within within our field. It's nice to sit with
a with a with a colleague, you know, you know,
someone who's in the same field as me. We can
talk shop. You know. Some people have criticized the ACE
(05:44):
research as too simplistic and and and some said, well,
some stuff that's replicated. Can you kind of give me
the latest if there is any consensus at all in
the field what what do you see as the as
the most as the guts consensus about the ACE research
and the extent to which particularly it causes long lasting
(06:06):
extent to what This is the key question, right, The
extent to which ace's early in childhood call cause long
lasting changes in the brain that make it very very
hard to change in adulthood.
Speaker 1 (06:16):
Yeah.
Speaker 2 (06:17):
Good.
Speaker 1 (06:18):
So, I think where I like to start is that
I think the work that was in some sense published
and brought to the four in nineteen ninety eight by
doctor Vincent Felidi and his colleagues put on the map
the importance of adverse childhood experiences. It wasn't that people
(06:39):
were not aware of the fact that adversity can shape development.
Speaker 2 (06:43):
I mean, this is an old topic.
Speaker 1 (06:45):
But the fact that it was brought to the four
in some sense for the medical community, who to this
day not necessarily asking about someone's childhood experiences, that was
in some sense the shock to fully experience that he
had never bothered to ask in his obesity clinic whether
(07:06):
adversity was an issue, and it was brought up to
the fore by a patient who said, I had been
sexually abused as a child. And so then he finds
of course that eighty percent of the population and morbially
abse people in his population were sexually abused incredibly, and
that was a shock to.
Speaker 3 (07:20):
Assist that, I'm shocked right now. That's not the general population.
Speaker 2 (07:24):
No, not general population.
Speaker 1 (07:25):
This but but, and this is important is he then
goes and presents this work and this is not in
the eighties.
Speaker 2 (07:31):
He goes and presents this work.
Speaker 1 (07:33):
To a group of medical people who are in the
eating disorder area, so they're professionals, right, And he's effectively
laughed off the stage. He said, come on, these people
are making stories up because they're overweight.
Speaker 2 (07:46):
That's you. Don't believe that to you.
Speaker 1 (07:48):
And then that night at dinner, a man from the
CDC is say next to him says, look, I don't
know if you're right or wrong.
Speaker 3 (07:53):
Why would overly people logically be more likely to make
up stories than non overly That.
Speaker 1 (07:57):
Was the impression of the doctors that if they're coming
up with an excuse rather than they got an eighty
year and excuse it.
Speaker 2 (08:03):
They're trying to excuse it away.
Speaker 3 (08:04):
See I see.
Speaker 1 (08:05):
And so the CDC guy says, look, I don't know
if you're right or wrong, but if you're right, we've
got a real medical problem on our hands, and we
need to find out. This is how the Ace's study
was initially born. So they you know, test seventeen thousand people,
not just the obese population obviously, and they find lo
and behold that aces are common, and they find that
(08:29):
there's a strong correlation with some of the biggest health
risks known to mankind.
Speaker 3 (08:34):
Right.
Speaker 1 (08:36):
That then quickly moves into the hands of the World
Health Organization, which sort of spreads out this questionnaire globally
and fast forward let's say till twenty ten, twenty twenty
or so, and we find that the following stats.
Speaker 2 (08:52):
The World Health.
Speaker 1 (08:53):
Organization reports about a year ago that globally, annually one billion.
Speaker 2 (08:59):
Children are mal treated every.
Speaker 1 (09:01):
Year in where global across the world, we have eight
billion people one eighth of those children, there's more children
of it. That's a mind numbing stat, okay, And that
and for me, you know, that's the kind of crushing
status that when we get these things please contribute to
something we want to hear about individuals. And for me,
(09:21):
of course, I experienced individuals who are like this daily.
So that's kind of the replication issue at some level.
That these are not part of dictatorships or democracies, or
heavily religious or non religious, or educated or not.
Speaker 2 (09:38):
These are global phenomenon.
Speaker 1 (09:39):
Okay, where I think things have gotten into trouble. And
this brings into the four is there's been a blending
of aces with its effects. But aces, as the acronum says,
adverse childhood experience. It's not the response to the experience.
(10:00):
So it's often confused with trauma. But trauma is the response.
Speaker 3 (10:05):
I love that you said that, and I would even
go one step further and say something a bit controversial.
It's the story that you created based on the response
to a real experience that happen to you.
Speaker 2 (10:16):
A story, and what I would add to it.
Speaker 1 (10:18):
And this is a distinction I make in the book
is that from adverse child experiences, some respond with trauma
and some resond resiliently.
Speaker 3 (10:26):
People I think George Bonano has found most people are
resilient and some even dare we say, have post traumatic growth.
Speaker 2 (10:33):
Completely, So there can be strength of growing out it.
Speaker 1 (10:35):
And we need to understand both what are the past
that leads some to be more vulnerable and others to
be more resilient. And part of What the book does
is it pulls from the science is to try to
understand better what the sciences is telling us about that
spectrum from vulnerable to resilience, which you can move across,
everybody can, but our biology is going to hand us
(10:57):
some piece that's going to put us somewhere in that spectacle.
Speaker 2 (11:00):
Said that, and then we're gonna move.
Speaker 3 (11:02):
So the dirty word the G word, you know, In
my new book I'm working on, I have a section
called the dirty G word, Yes, because it's amazing to
me the extend to which the trait neuroticism colors your
world and even makes it more likely that you'll you'll
label things as traumas that someone who scores low neuroticism is,
(11:22):
like I wasn't traumatizing. You can have two people in
the same family, right, one who scores high scores low,
have the same experiences in childhood being treated the same
by the parent, and then you you can find gross
differences in one person's interpretation of their childhood is traumatic.
And that's interesting, isn't it. Talking about it. I'm even
talking about like fraternal twins. Oh completely, yeah.
Speaker 2 (11:45):
That's right. And I think.
Speaker 1 (11:48):
Different experiences can also shape how we respond. I mean,
there's a lovely kind of strategy that's often used in schools,
but it should be actually used by anybody, which is
it's goes by the name the size of the problem.
So the idea is, imagine three boxes of different sizes,
a tiny one.
Speaker 2 (12:05):
A middle size one, and a big one.
Speaker 1 (12:07):
Okay, So when you're in a calm state, what would
you consider to be a small problem, right, And so
you might model it and say, well, you know, I
bang my toe against the wall.
Speaker 2 (12:17):
It hurts, but you know it's temporary. Tiny problem. Big problem.
Speaker 1 (12:21):
My mom's got cancer and is unlikely to survive, okay
for me, painful experience.
Speaker 3 (12:27):
That's so.
Speaker 2 (12:28):
And then the middle is I've got a deadline.
Speaker 1 (12:30):
I'm going to go to a podcast with Scott and
the traffic is killing me and I'm stressing out. I'm
not gonna make it on time. It's kind of middling
sized problems. I have no control over it, which is
also stressful. The thing is that as you experience different
kinds of things in the world, those boxes can get reshaped,
as you see as for example, for me working in Kenya,
(12:52):
where there's three million orphans in the world, and I
see their experience. I go, my problems are never in
kind of the big box relative to.
Speaker 3 (12:59):
Those kids you've worked with.
Speaker 1 (13:02):
So for the last three years we haven't. We actually
have a nonprofit called International.
Speaker 3 (13:05):
Do they actually look like what the commercials will make
them look like?
Speaker 1 (13:08):
Yeah?
Speaker 3 (13:08):
No, okay, talk to me.
Speaker 2 (13:10):
So we'll talked about that.
Speaker 1 (13:11):
So sub Saharan Africa, so you know, the last third
of the continent has fifty four million orphans, some of
the biggest populations orphans in the world. Kenya itself, a
country of about fifty million people, has three million.
Speaker 3 (13:26):
They put them all.
Speaker 2 (13:28):
So these are so they're.
Speaker 1 (13:30):
Almost about a thousand orphanages at Kenya, not the government, right, okay, okay.
And so one of the things that we're doing is
trying to bring resources to these places. Because I'm sure
we'll get back to this. Part of the story about
neglect and deprivation grows out of the incredible scientific work
out of Romania with the Romanian orphanages, and and part
(13:51):
as a result of that work, virtually all the orphanages
in Eastern Europe were shut down because they were basically institutions.
There were incredibly deprived children. As the nineteen nineties Shame
of Nation documentary pointed out they're like Nazi concentration camps
for little kids. The difference is, it's important, is that
in many low to middle income countries, these orphanages, which
(14:14):
in canadayre called children's.
Speaker 2 (14:15):
Homes, are very different.
Speaker 1 (14:18):
You don't see kids who are gaunt and unhealthy and
look like they're not going to be able to walk
two steps. And in part it's because you find these
long lived staff who stay with these kids, who become
the nurturing parents effectively and enable those kids to grow up.
Speaker 2 (14:33):
But the funding is.
Speaker 1 (14:34):
Coming from churches, from donors. It's really shoe string kind
of budget.
Speaker 3 (14:41):
Well, thank you, you're hallucinating me so much right now.
I don't think most of US Americans know any of
this shit. They don't and we're blind to it. And
maybe a lot more people that have about more compassion
if they we could take a trip over there.
Speaker 2 (14:55):
I think that's right.
Speaker 1 (14:56):
And I think you know, part of the angle of
the book, which I know you're aware of the psychologist,
is that you know, psych cology has had this experience
that our population of study tends to be this weird,
distorted population in some sense, and part of the angle
of my book, which I think is quite different from
most books on therapeutic traumatic issues, is it has a
very strong cross cultural lens that I'm talking about our species.
Speaker 3 (15:18):
At some level.
Speaker 1 (15:19):
And I think that's important because strategies to help children
have to be cross culturally sensitive.
Speaker 3 (15:31):
Well, that was actually gonna be my next question with
I love the extent to which you really your book
is different from a lot of other books in trauma
by taking this cross cultural lens. But you know, American culture,
let's talk about America's obsession with trauma. Yes, because there's
TikTok trauma, which I think might be different than how
psychologists define trauma. But now you have you know, every
(15:55):
back pain, every back pain, every just just every neurotic
thought is now a trauma. First of all, do you agree, like,
do you see it kind of getting a little bit
out of control and far outside the realm of what
we would call trauma.
Speaker 2 (16:09):
I do.
Speaker 1 (16:09):
I think the word has become part of the coloquial language.
It's become part of the common use of words, and
I think it's interesting.
Speaker 2 (16:18):
I began to get your insights into this too. As
a psychologist.
Speaker 1 (16:22):
I think a lot of times in the science is
when words that have a technical definition become part of
the language, science tends to retreat and become almost more technical,
so that the layperson can't access the information. In some ways,
my book is an attempt to kind of address that,
because the gap in terms of what we understand the
(16:43):
sciences and what practitioners understand is monumental.
Speaker 2 (16:47):
So sadly, I think you're right that in.
Speaker 1 (16:50):
This country, especially, the word trauma has become part of
the common lore.
Speaker 2 (16:56):
People use it.
Speaker 1 (16:58):
I think I described in the book. I had a
friend of mine who I saw downtown living in California.
He looked really out of sorts, and I said, hey,
you know what's going on.
Speaker 2 (17:08):
He goes, it's the worst day of my life. I said, well,
what happened?
Speaker 1 (17:10):
He goes, my son's soccer urnmost canceled. I'm like, if
that's your worst day, I want your life.
Speaker 2 (17:16):
So that's yeah, none zero.
Speaker 3 (17:19):
Right.
Speaker 2 (17:20):
So, so of.
Speaker 1 (17:21):
Course the term trauma in some ways, well it's Greek origin,
but PTSD grew out of kind of the early sixties
and the war veterans who were initially called just crazy,
and then of course it becomes part of the DSM
in terms of PTSD, and it has this technical term.
And what's interesting there and thing especially for you as
(17:42):
a psychologist, is there's been some push of late, even
of the DSM terminology and for the reason of thinking
about children.
Speaker 2 (17:50):
So let me give you an example.
Speaker 1 (17:54):
If you fall under the classifications of mental health issues
in the that then licenses you to insurance coverage.
Speaker 2 (18:05):
If you don't, it doesn't, so it's now out of pocket.
Speaker 1 (18:09):
There are now many, many cases of children who have
significant traumatic responses. You can see it physiologically, neurobiologically, but
they don't fall under the criteria of PTSD. There are
several psychiatrists and psychologists, Besil Vanderkoke being one, who have
(18:31):
argued for a new diagnosis, which is called developmental trauma disorder.
Speaker 2 (18:37):
Which is in particular for children.
Speaker 1 (18:38):
And here's the reason why I think it's important. I
discussed this in the book. PTSD in the traditional sense
is due to exposure to violence. Oh, a lot of
traumatic responses are not necessarily due to violence. Children who
have been severely deprived are heavily traumatized, and yet they
(18:59):
don't count. Neglect is neglectsidered out, attachment issues, right, abandonments, right,
But they're not in necessarily violent.
Speaker 2 (19:09):
And so they've been trying now for probably ten.
Speaker 1 (19:12):
Years to get DTD developmental trauma disorder into the CM,
but the powers that be have rejected it, even though
they've followed the guilence for what counts as getting something in.
Speaker 2 (19:24):
So there's been a lot of controversies. I'm sure you're aware.
Speaker 1 (19:26):
Of that the DSM either puts in too many things
or not enough.
Speaker 3 (19:31):
Oh. I got a lot of issues with the d
also that it treats everything binary. Yes, you either have
the disorder or not. And you know, I'm friends with
the task force trying to show the continuum that all
these things are on. But you know, I'm glad you
brought up Bessel. Bessel who was author of The Body
(19:52):
Keeps the Score. You know, I of course have a
great respect for his long career, and he's done a
lot the field, but I kind of see the ideas
in his book, or even just the notion that the
body keeps the score. Let's double click on that a second.
It's run a muck in our society and and people
don't realize that that's not actually how it works. So
(20:16):
the brain, you like, like trauma's not like let's talk
about memory for a second. Memory is not stored in
the body. His memory comes from the brain, and we
can have I prefer the term survival stress. And I
wanted to see your thoughts because I do like to
give it in a nuanced a way. I'm not, like,
you know, trying to tear down. I'm trying to have
(20:36):
more nuance around the idea, because it's too simplistic to
have this notion that, like the collective memory, the memory
of our trauma is stored in our body and we
can't our brain can't access it, and we're doomed forever.
To me, that's a disempowering message. I think the truth
is actually more empowering. Thext into which top down processes,
you know, really can help influence our physiology and ours.
(20:59):
There are a lot of techniques, you know. I'm a
big fan of psychodrama, for instance, you know good my
friend Dan Thomasulo is a leader in that he's here
in New York City, So shout out to Dantell. But so,
I just think there's a kind of a disempowering message
going around about trauma that it's like it's there's unseen
force within your body that is causing everything you do,
(21:20):
and you really there's really not that much hope in
being able to act sin, you know, because it's in
your body, not your mind. But as you know, George
Bonano told me over coffee once. I don't know a
lot to repeat everything he said to me over coffee,
but he's like, if you've had trauma, you remember it
like it's in your brain. It's not it's not hit.
(21:42):
If you've experienced trauma, it's not hidden. I mean, we're
over that whole, you know, Elizabeth loftis you know, Craze
in the I mean, I don't want to go back there,
you know, you know what I'm saying.
Speaker 1 (21:54):
I do know. So so let's let's pull apart some
of these pieces because I think they're they're interesting in themselves.
So I think it's a kind of a strong version
of the Body keeps the Score, which I think many
have interpreted as I think there's a weaker version, which
is the more interesting version. The strong version is what
(22:17):
you've painted, which is that there's a memory my kneecap
for you know, seeing my mom.
Speaker 2 (22:22):
Get hit hit by a car.
Speaker 1 (22:23):
Well, that's clearly false because there is no memory system
in my recap.
Speaker 2 (22:27):
But there's a more interesting version, which is that there's
a body.
Speaker 3 (22:32):
Response survival stress.
Speaker 1 (22:34):
Survival stress which includes the autonomic and the immune system.
And I think why I think that is critically important
is because of two reasons. One, sometimes what we see
operationally in an individual cognitively suggests no trauma, but the
(22:59):
body is hammered.
Speaker 2 (23:01):
So here's a perfect example.
Speaker 1 (23:03):
This was This is a case that Felidi himself, you know,
talked about. Several years after he had published the original paper,
a woman contacted him and said, look, I'm.
Speaker 2 (23:12):
So glad to see this report.
Speaker 1 (23:15):
You know, I am today a Seventh Court Circuit judge.
So clearly on every cognitive level, she has made it right.
Speaker 2 (23:23):
She's in her fifties, she was.
Speaker 1 (23:26):
Pimped out by her father a kid sexually year after
year after year.
Speaker 2 (23:30):
So this is one ace. Now let's keep that in
mind too, because.
Speaker 3 (23:33):
We'll come back to that face out should count.
Speaker 2 (23:35):
Well.
Speaker 1 (23:36):
Let's yeah, let's come back to that, right, because because
the A score is about a number of different types,
pretty intense, intense, so we'll.
Speaker 2 (23:42):
Come back to that.
Speaker 1 (23:43):
Let's let's remember to come back to that she was
sitting on four different types of cancer. Her immune system
was completely destroyed, even though she was cognitively top of
her class.
Speaker 2 (23:57):
Judge, I mean, you know, you name it well.
Speaker 1 (23:59):
So I think the reason why I think that's important
is because her body has kept the score of the
traus stress in terms of the immune system, in the
same way we see epigenetic tracking of things, right.
Speaker 3 (24:12):
I was just gonna say that.
Speaker 1 (24:13):
So I think that's that's the it's the weaker but
the more interesting part.
Speaker 2 (24:17):
I agree, And that's where I.
Speaker 1 (24:19):
Think Besil vonakokes and if you you know, I've been
to many of the conferences he has in Boston.
Speaker 2 (24:25):
And the group.
Speaker 1 (24:26):
The heavy focus is on things like you know, emdr
you know I I things and yoga and breathing. It's
all body work because there is a sense in which
there's a scarring.
Speaker 2 (24:40):
To the body.
Speaker 1 (24:41):
Of course, there is scarring to the brain too, and
that's what the memory is held. But I think we've
not given enough attention to what's happening the body. And
men and the children I work with directly, they have
no body awareness. They're talking to you, and what's happening
is I can see their fiscally like and they have
(25:02):
no somatic awareness.
Speaker 3 (25:05):
Yeah, I get it. I get it, I really do.
I wonder how how necessary it is for you know
the word to think of it in terms of it's
because of that one thing, you know, that that that trauma,
as opposed to well, this is the way my body
is right now, and let's change it, let's work on it.
But I feel like there's this people, there's something in
(25:27):
human nature that wants to understand why am I the
way I am? And I always get a little I
don't know, I wary when people want to reduce everything
to that one event, to that one thing that happens,
the one thing. You know, the reason why I'm an
asshole to everyone around me is because of that one thing. Yes,
I get I get a little worry about that, you know,
(25:49):
because we're so we're so multi determined for systems.
Speaker 1 (25:53):
So okay, So that that's a nice segue into this
kind of one ACE idea. So you raised the question
at the beginning of controversial pieces of the Ace's story.
Speaker 2 (26:05):
So the first one we've kind of tackled, which is
that there's been a confusion about the.
Speaker 1 (26:08):
Experience versus the response. So we've kind of we'll come
back to that, but that's a key one.
Speaker 2 (26:12):
The second is that the ACE score.
Speaker 1 (26:17):
Is somehow indicative of the individual's potential problems. And there's
two problems with that. The first is the originators of
the ACE questionnaire, which was ten questions about different types
of adverse childhood experiences. That was never intended as a
questionnaire about the individual.
Speaker 2 (26:39):
It's populating.
Speaker 3 (26:41):
That was supposed to be about population into different populations
and get a pulse on correct.
Speaker 1 (26:45):
Oh, I didn't know that, Okay, And that's really important
because in California, for example, it's been used by insurance
to coverage.
Speaker 2 (26:56):
If you have an ACE.
Speaker 1 (26:57):
Score of four or more, you get a coverage which
is different than less than four. Has nothing to do
with the individual though.
Speaker 3 (27:02):
That just blew my mind, yepow.
Speaker 1 (27:04):
And we know from the science that the individual score
is not predictive of what's going to happen to them.
Speaker 3 (27:10):
Well, and that's the key thing that's a seeing criticized.
I've seen very weak correlations.
Speaker 1 (27:14):
Oh, it's really week and it's weaking two interesting ways.
One is the ASS questionnaire is a retrospective questionnaire. What
happened to you Scott as a child?
Speaker 3 (27:23):
Yeah, And that's so problematic and we know that's problematic,
so we know just how memory, how valuable memory is completely.
Speaker 1 (27:30):
So that's problem number one. The second problem is it
assumes that the number of types is an accurate lens
on health risks as opposed to what I bring up
in my book, which is different dimensions of adversities.
Speaker 2 (27:45):
Let's take apart some of these.
Speaker 1 (27:47):
The first is that they operationally define childhood as birth
to eighteen.
Speaker 2 (27:53):
That's a pretty big swath of time. As a psychologist,
you're well aware.
Speaker 1 (27:57):
That there are these different sensitive and critical periods for
different things, and they're happening at different ages within that span.
Just to give one example of something that's recent that
we all experience, COVID hits.
Speaker 2 (28:12):
Who does it hit hardest teenagers? Why?
Speaker 1 (28:15):
Because the teenage years are a critical period for social interactions.
Speaker 2 (28:19):
They were deprived for two years.
Speaker 1 (28:21):
Of social interactions. Social interactions to teenagers who are like
milk to a baby, They're needed and this has been
well documented scientifically. So you have globally this massive response
by teenagers to that deprivation. The mental health crisis, and
I know you've had guests that have talked about this
has been pushed higher because of what COVID did to
(28:43):
that teenage group. This is irrespective of cell phone, social
media and all the rest. It just bumped it up.
We are seeing kids in school now who, let's say,
are chronologically fourteen, who are actually developmentally like nine.
Speaker 3 (28:56):
Hold on, so my brain goes to, like the intelligence
quot and how it was originally scored. What would we
call this quotient? Yeah, it's an interesting question.
Speaker 1 (29:04):
I mean, it's probably close to the EQ, but it's
probably like seq social emotional question question right.
Speaker 3 (29:09):
Where you are emotionally developed? How mostly developed are you?
Speaker 2 (29:12):
Yeah?
Speaker 1 (29:13):
I mean I mean think think about you know, theory
of mind, you know, or related to Interrelli's beliefs, you know, intentions, desires,
and so forth.
Speaker 2 (29:19):
These kids were deprived of those critical periods.
Speaker 3 (29:22):
This is explain the crybabies on campus right now.
Speaker 2 (29:26):
Yes, an over exaggeration at babies. Oh wait, well well
that's that's.
Speaker 1 (29:30):
Actually it's funny and it's also really potentially poignant because
the Adverse Childhood Experiences questionnaire stops at eighteen, but we
know developmentally that that frontel is continuing twenty five exactly.
And the other piece of it that I'm going to
add in today is that pre natally, we know that
(29:52):
stress to the mother directly affects the developing fetus. And
there was a study that was published last year, actually
fascinating study that was done with moms who were exposed
to Hurricane Sandy in New Jersey, and they compared it
with mothers who had their babies kind of before versus
(30:14):
mothers kind of well afterwards.
Speaker 2 (30:16):
But here's what they found.
Speaker 1 (30:18):
They followed those babies up the age of five. Girls
who were in the womb during Hurricane Sandy had five
to six times higher rates of anxiety and depression and
boys had six times higher ADHD in opposition of defiance
because of the stress of the mom.
Speaker 3 (30:39):
Well that's super interesting, right. I wanted to actually double
cook on their gender differences. So I'm glad you kind
of brought them up, because in the beginning you brought PTSD,
and you brought up numbers like one billion have been
but I actually want to break it down by gender
a little bit. Yeah, I continually amazed when I look
at the striking gender differences in peak SD. It's women
(31:02):
really do disproportionately report higher levels of PTSD than men.
Men have their own issues, Don't get me wrong. You know,
like there are things with ADHD, we can autism, you know,
we can bring up we all got issues. But but
what do you think what do you think is going
on there? Though? Like why why? Why is that the case?
Speaker 1 (31:22):
So yeah, as a general cut, when you look at
the kind of the mental health landscape, the broad again,
sex differences a lot of variation. But as a broad cut,
girls women tend to be more internalizers, boys men externalizers,
and we certainly see that a lot.
Speaker 2 (31:45):
But what cross cuts some of that.
Speaker 1 (31:50):
Is also the type of adversity. For example, it's certainly
not the case that boys are immune sexual abuse, as
we know from the church scandals. That's important, right, But
a lot of the understanding of sexual abuse has come
primarily or more often from.
Speaker 3 (32:11):
The female side, right.
Speaker 1 (32:14):
So for example, this is an incredible study that was
done out of Germany. They showed that women who were
abused as children before the age of puberty as opposed
to after, there's an area in the brain in the
somatosensory cortex that maps to the genitals glitteris and females
(32:40):
that area is smaller in girls who were sexually abused
before puberty.
Speaker 3 (32:45):
What's the implication of that for their own lived experience
growing up.
Speaker 1 (32:48):
As so as adults sexually averse painful experiences during sex
pleasures pleasure see.
Speaker 2 (32:56):
Which has been documented.
Speaker 1 (32:57):
But now you've got this neuroanatomical landmark. And as you
I'm sure no, the amount of stensity cortex in some
ways is a map of kind of the body and
the extent to which it's innervated. So for example, our
lips and our fingers have a massive space, and that's
the area our kneecaps do not right, and so the clitorist,
obviously a heavily innervated area, is basically shrunk in girls
(33:20):
who are before puberty. Again pointing to that timing issue.
It's not just adversity and type, but the timing.
Speaker 3 (33:27):
Yeah, so I transcend I write about you know, Titra's
research t I c HS and some other research showing
so clearly how depending on what kind of way you're maltreated,
the brain has very specific ways it organizes itself to
protect you from the emotional pain of it. But I
went down a list of things, so you mentioned one,
(33:49):
but we can we can mention other things, many types
emotional abuse exactly, And the migdala does correct. Yeah.
Speaker 1 (33:56):
Yeah. So the lens that I use in the book
as a framework is what I called the adverse ts
T for letter T, because there are five different dimensions
of adversity. One we've already talked about the type of adversity,
The second is timing. The third is tenure how long
it lasts.
Speaker 3 (34:13):
To be confused with academic.
Speaker 2 (34:16):
Tenure, which.
Speaker 1 (34:19):
The fourth, uh is what I call turbulence, which is
the predictability or controllability.
Speaker 3 (34:24):
Of the adversity.
Speaker 2 (34:25):
And the fifth is toxicity.
Speaker 3 (34:27):
Oh boy, what would non toxic abuse be.
Speaker 1 (34:30):
Well, there's not, so there's not there's no toxicity, but
sever and And you know, one of the things I
bring up in the book, which is kind of maybe
topical today a little bit, is during the Me Too movement,
Matt Damn is coming, you know, don't we want to
distinguish between you know, a poorly time sexual joke and rape.
And he got taken the town for that. But of
(34:52):
course we do, because it makes a difference in terms
of how the body responds. Body and brain respond totally
differently to a poor sexually time joke, which can be
certainly aversive, versus being raped.
Speaker 3 (35:05):
Yeah, I mean, I think that like in a lot
of ways, trauma is in the eye of the beholder.
I mean someone who you could imagine someone's experience, lived
experience being who has been raped and who is at
a comedy club and here's a rape joke, and to
that person in their unit, like everyone else around him
like ah, but in her or his universe, like they're
(35:29):
feeling an panic. And I guess in some ways, a
lot of it does come down to just like honoring
someone's experience of something. And maybe that makes me sound
really woke to say such a thing, but I am
a big believer in you know that kind of Let
there be a little subjectivity here where like I don't
know what it's like for someone else to experience. I
(35:53):
wouldn't even downplay or make fun of someone who has
a visceral reaction to a joke that I find fun. Yes,
I would probably still honor that person has that experience. Yes,
I guess I would want them to also honor that
I don't have that experience. So maybe it works both ways,
is my point. It does.
Speaker 1 (36:09):
No, that's a very nice point, and it brings up
something that's both a practical issue and then kind of
an empirical experimental issue on the practical level, of course,
and this is this is the comedy about the woke
piece is that you know everything's got a trigger warning.
Now everything does right, And that's a course problematic in
and of itself. That said, as someone who works with
(36:31):
children with significant traumatic experiences, one of my first experiences
this actually happened the first year I started doing this work.
Speaker 2 (36:40):
I walk into a middle.
Speaker 1 (36:41):
School and this little boy both side of the classroom
as like, I don't think I did anything, and the
teacher comes up, a lovely teacher and puts her on
my shoulder and says, look, I'm sorry, it's not you.
Speaker 2 (36:53):
You haven't just looked like his father, who beats him
almost every night.
Speaker 3 (36:56):
Eat.
Speaker 2 (36:57):
So I was the trigger.
Speaker 1 (36:58):
Okay, next time I can in the classroom, I sat
down on the ground and looked away from him. So
I was smaller than him and I wasn't paying attention
to him. And then he comes up to me and goes,
what's matter. You're not gonna talk to me. But now
I kind of calm the unsafe environment down, okay, which
is he needs to learn not to generalize that all
men are not going to be abusive. So it's kind
(37:18):
of the reversal typical psychology where we want generalization for learning.
With him, it's taking generalization away so he can feel safe.
Speaker 3 (37:26):
Well, that's wonderful, patients, you had there to do that.
You know, fear learning and fear unlearning are different, operating
in different parts of the brain, different and so we
have to learn.
Speaker 1 (37:36):
So let me, let me just give me just let
me just then the second piece, because I think it's
critical what you asked.
Speaker 2 (37:41):
The toxicity level.
Speaker 1 (37:42):
Of course, at some level it is subjective. But here's
where I think we've made progress. You know what you
and I might consider to be neglect of a parent,
and then you look at what's happening to these orphans.
Speaker 2 (37:56):
It's night and day, right.
Speaker 1 (37:59):
However, one of the things that's come out of, for example,
the word comparing the Romanian orphan children, which was really
severe toxic at the highest level deprivation. They had nothing,
no stable staff, poorly nutritious food, constrain, living quarters, I mean,
you name it.
Speaker 2 (38:16):
It was the worst on a spectrum.
Speaker 1 (38:19):
But we know that socioeconomic scales mean something, and you
can map people onto those and you can see the
effects on grades and attendance and all these other things.
So there's an important piece that dimension and experimental work
on animals, for example, has been able to experimentally tightrate
that to see what's kind of the minimal experience that
(38:39):
one would need. And of course, the now thought of
as horrible experiments by.
Speaker 2 (38:43):
Harry Harlow were those experiments.
Speaker 1 (38:46):
I mean, we may not like them ethically, which I
certainly point out in the book, but they did try
to tight trate what's needed for developing monkey to have
functional social relationships. That's what the experiment was up tight
trading deprivation.
Speaker 3 (39:04):
I mean, that was that was the part of your
career was studying, right, monkeys. You were a professor at
Harvard and you studied. You were a big name in
the stud morality. Right, you studied morality. You stayed monkeys.
What happened? Something happened at Harvard? What happened, What happened?
What happened to you?
Speaker 2 (39:21):
What happened? What happened?
Speaker 1 (39:23):
Yeah, So you know, this is something that I really
haven't talked about.
Speaker 2 (39:27):
And you know, my motivation for not talking about.
Speaker 1 (39:32):
The misconduct cakes, which I do mention at the front
of the book, was designed really in some ways by
my part to protect my students.
Speaker 2 (39:42):
I had a very, very large lab. I wanted to
make sure that what was ever.
Speaker 1 (39:46):
Happening to me went on for five years while I
was at Harvard, that it didn't hurt them. And you know,
I'm glad to say that all those students went on
to have tremendous careers.
Speaker 3 (39:57):
So it is the implicit assumption there. So you're saying
you didn't forge data yourself.
Speaker 1 (40:02):
So what I own as the head of a lab
was that I was trying to do too much. I
was not as careful as I should have been.
Speaker 3 (40:14):
Okay, So it's unintentional, is what you're saying.
Speaker 2 (40:16):
Unintentional?
Speaker 3 (40:17):
That's my direct question. Was it into unintentional?
Speaker 2 (40:19):
Intentional?
Speaker 3 (40:19):
She didn't sit down one day and you're like, let
me see how I can finance the state and finances
and finesse finesse the data that.
Speaker 2 (40:26):
You didn't do that I did not.
Speaker 3 (40:27):
That's that's a big difference.
Speaker 2 (40:28):
It is a big difference.
Speaker 1 (40:29):
And you know, I I want to just note that
when I think of the potential damage that misconduct Kennon
has done.
Speaker 2 (40:41):
I think of two things.
Speaker 1 (40:43):
One, it causes researchers to invest time in looking at
things that are false, and it, in course, it wastes
taxpayer money grants. In my case, the case ended and
I had to retract to one paper. Several years after
(41:04):
that paper was retracted, two independent labs replicated the actual result.
Speaker 2 (41:09):
So I wasn't involved in those.
Speaker 1 (41:11):
They did them themselves, and so the you know, the
the result that we had was wrong because of the
experimental design was wrong and it led to a false conclusion.
But the actual result was actually replicated by two and.
Speaker 3 (41:24):
So that's just like a human to human question. Do
you feel like the punishment was too harsh for the
quote crime? Because because your career, right took a big hit.
I mean, this must be emotional for you, right, because
I mean, you're a human who went through this. You're
not just some sort of abstract entity. They were like, oh,
look at Marca, I mean, what what was it? What
(41:45):
has it been like for you? Like I really care?
Speaker 2 (41:48):
Yeah, no, thank you for the question. I appreciate it.
Speaker 1 (41:51):
So I guess the first thing to say is that
I resigned.
Speaker 3 (41:56):
You weren't actually fired at the end of.
Speaker 1 (41:58):
At the end of the at the end of the investigation,
the dean at the time said, you know, this has
not been your most shining moment, but you are still
member of our faculty. And then, for a variety of reasons,
it just became not a place where I could really work,
and for my own emotional sanity and for my family
(42:20):
and my students.
Speaker 3 (42:21):
Did you have like colleagues and friends who kind of
turned on you? Or yes, he just lost who I lost? Friends?
Speaker 1 (42:27):
Who I lost his friends. However, as you probably know,
and you know the back of the book, some of
my closest friends and colleagues, Steve Pinker and Susan Carey,
stat as friends and they've blurbed the book, and I,
you know, and I maintain friendships. And since that time happened,
some colleagues asked me to come back to academia, and
(42:49):
I decided I did not want to, in part because
I had found this second chapter, which to me, to
be totally honest, I probably wouldn't have found it because
I simply I had plans of being an old part
professor at Harvard rest of my life.
Speaker 3 (43:00):
You know what you had, postraumatic growth? Would you would
you agree that that's what?
Speaker 2 (43:05):
Yes, I do.
Speaker 1 (43:06):
I do, and I certainly feel that in interesting ways,
I didn't think I'd find something that I.
Speaker 2 (43:13):
Could do meaningfully that could use some of my skills.
Speaker 3 (43:17):
Yeah.
Speaker 1 (43:17):
Yeah, but it turned out that almost all of my
skills have paid off in this whole new area.
Speaker 3 (43:22):
How did the morality work you did, then, How does
that play a role in the work you do today
at all? Or is it? Are they really complete separate
chapters of your life.
Speaker 2 (43:31):
They're not.
Speaker 1 (43:32):
They're not completely separate because and I'll just give you
a couple of examples. One of the consequences of certain
kinds of traumatic experience, in particular things like sexual abuse
and physical abuse, is that it brings very strong moral
emotions to the victims shame.
Speaker 3 (43:51):
Oh, that's true. That's true.
Speaker 1 (43:53):
And I think having a relatively richer understanding of emotions,
how they work, feelings, moral emotions, moral psychology has helped
in my conversations with kids and with and with the
staff who work with them, teachers, counselors, clinicians, doctors. So
it's informed how I speak to the individual I work with,
(44:16):
which includes the kids.
Speaker 2 (44:19):
And maybe it's had a height level of compassion for others.
I think, you know.
Speaker 1 (44:25):
In one of the podcasts I was on, it was
kind of reframed as second chances.
Speaker 2 (44:30):
All the kids I work with are second chance kids.
Speaker 3 (44:33):
I love this. Well, this is personal to you too.
You're a second chance. I'm giving you a second chance
right now completely by having you on the second of
So let's talk about hope. Let's let's spend the rest
of this interview today talking about this toolkit that you've
developed and and and have you really seen examples firsthand
of huge transformations and some of these kids in the
(44:54):
way that they there their their sort of prospects in
their head for their future.
Speaker 2 (44:59):
Yes, let me illustrate one. I'll give you two.
Speaker 1 (45:07):
This was a charming story in many ways.
Speaker 2 (45:09):
So I'm in this class. I'm teaching a class.
Speaker 1 (45:12):
So I've in a lot of the programs I work
and I teach a class on critical thinking because to me,
as kids, we need this.
Speaker 2 (45:17):
They need this. They act too impulsively.
Speaker 1 (45:22):
They don't stop before they think, you know, and think
that they do. So I'm in his class and we're
talking about similarities different between humans and other animals. And
we've talked about the similarities and we and then the
question that gets posted this class about ten or so
middle school aged kids about ten to twelve years old,
is what's uniquely human? And this one boy leaps out
(45:43):
of his chair.
Speaker 2 (45:46):
And says sentience.
Speaker 1 (45:48):
And I'm like, so, I say to him, did you
say sentience or sentence?
Speaker 2 (45:55):
He says, what the fuck? Mark? Why would I say sentence?
Not even an answer to the question, Like, okaya, my bad,
I'm old, I'm old. I can't hear that.
Speaker 3 (46:02):
Well that's incredible.
Speaker 2 (46:04):
Okay, so it gets better.
Speaker 3 (46:06):
That's a future Steven Pinker right there.
Speaker 1 (46:08):
So I say, okay, so, my bad, I said, does
anybody here know what what sentience means.
Speaker 2 (46:14):
In their crickets? I said, Charlie, can you uh tell
us what sentience?
Speaker 1 (46:20):
Music goes rolls his eyes, Well, okay, you know, well,
it's like kind of the experience of what it's like
to experience, Like I can't really know what it's like
to experience what you're experiencing sort of, really, I really
can't know what it's like to experience what other.
Speaker 2 (46:31):
Animals are experiencing. But yeah, it's the feeling of experience
that's really good. I said, where did you learn about it?
Speaker 1 (46:36):
He goes, you know, YouTube, those guys, I think, I
don't if they're philosophers or scientists, but like, you know,
Pinker Dennett Chalmers.
Speaker 2 (46:43):
I'm like, and my jaw drops. He's like, what haven't
you read them?
Speaker 1 (46:50):
This is a kid complete deprivation, neglect at home, parents,
with mental health issues.
Speaker 2 (46:57):
But through this program.
Speaker 1 (46:59):
Of people believing in him, now he had the biology
of an incredible brain, but deprived and neglected.
Speaker 2 (47:07):
Comes in filthy, doesn't wash. School does as best they can,
but the school is there day after day. I could
give him books.
Speaker 1 (47:18):
He has grown over the last three years. He's now fourteen, thirteen, fourteen,
he's taking the high school class I'm giving on human
rights and civics, and he's just blossoming because his trust
in the kids. So there are many many cases like that.
Similarly in Kenya where we're doing the work. Three years
(47:40):
ago I went to an orphanage. A little boy had
literally just been dropped off the doorstep, emaciated.
Speaker 2 (47:48):
Almost no clothes, mute. Of course, know the history was
about four and a half years of age.
Speaker 1 (47:55):
We came back this year to the same orphanage to
put in a library. Little boy is bright eyed, talking, reading.
Speaker 3 (48:04):
Playing, incredible.
Speaker 1 (48:06):
So those transformations happen, they're slow, they're hard. But for me,
where the next steps are is that a lot of
the ideas evidence that I cover in the book is
not in the hands of practitioners, and that's what's said, Well.
Speaker 3 (48:22):
What is the toolkit it consists of.
Speaker 2 (48:24):
Okay, so the.
Speaker 1 (48:26):
Toolkit is really, for me a broad swath of things
for a couple of reasons.
Speaker 2 (48:33):
One is that, like.
Speaker 1 (48:35):
Almost in anything in life, there's not one shoe fits all.
But the reason to really take that seriously is because
these five different dimensions of adversity create what I call
traumatic signatures. So in the same way that neuropsychologists have
uncovered ways in which when certain parts of the brain
are damaged, you get these signatures which indicate the cognitive deficit,
(48:59):
the ephasia's right prosobagnosia, faced deficit, and so forth. Traumatic
experiences can create other signatures that if we understand them,
we can better respond with specific traumatic toolkits.
Speaker 2 (49:13):
Okay, So that's the key idea. Well, just as a.
Speaker 1 (49:15):
Simple cut, there's many more dimensions. What abuse typically does
is it creates and you mentioned the term before, it
creates this hypervigiline amygdala.
Speaker 2 (49:27):
You are on high alert. The world is unsafe. Okay. So,
for example, that little boy who bolted when he saw me.
Speaker 1 (49:34):
He is like a gazelle on the savann who sees
a lion attack the group. The gazelle doesn't conclude that
lion's bad. They conclude all lions are bad, and that's
the right conclusion. Highly adaptive. Yeah, okay, so they've overgeneralized adaptively.
And what schools, therapists, parents, communities have to do is
(49:55):
help tamp down that amygdala so that the world doesn't
feel unsafe.
Speaker 2 (50:00):
So part of that.
Speaker 1 (50:01):
Work can be breathing methods, getting control over the breathing system,
the size of the problem, types of stuff, other kinds
of therapeutic work, so a whole variety of things to
help tamp.
Speaker 2 (50:14):
Down so they don't generalize. Compare that with what.
Speaker 1 (50:18):
Happens with the lovely little child I mentioned before sentience.
Speaker 2 (50:22):
He's deprived.
Speaker 1 (50:22):
He does not have a hyperactive emiglo. Deprivation undermines the
development of the funnel lobe and therefore the executive system,
so the systems for learning, attention, short term working memory,
self regulations. Yeah, that's right exactly. So he had a
system that was designed to learn the cortex active working absolutely,
(50:45):
but for him, the association of actions and consequences were undermined.
He wasn't getting the kind of feedback because the whole
reward system was not really wired.
Speaker 3 (50:54):
I see.
Speaker 1 (50:54):
So for them, it's developing those systems of self regulation.
He flipped when I say sentence right, completely emotionally justsregulated
in that sense, right. And attention wasn't great constantly looking around, right,
So very different systems. So how do we design these
systems so that we can nurture the pieces that need nurturing.
(51:16):
So part of it are the sort of these social
emotional breathing, meditative attentional work, sometimes medication, depending.
Speaker 2 (51:23):
On that extreme the case.
Speaker 3 (51:24):
Maybe.
Speaker 1 (51:25):
But one of the things I cover towards the end
of the book is what I call the neuroengineering approaches.
Speaker 2 (51:31):
And many of these are.
Speaker 1 (51:33):
Not yet ready for prime time for kids yet, but
they hold out hopes.
Speaker 2 (51:37):
Let me mention two.
Speaker 1 (51:41):
One of the techniques which has now been approved by
the FDA for depression is transcranial magnetic stimulation. So this
is kind of the magnet apply to the cortex exactly and.
Speaker 2 (51:55):
Exactly.
Speaker 1 (51:56):
Yes, And here's a result which I just love because
it shows how understanding brain function with new technology can
really begin to be moved.
Speaker 2 (52:08):
In these applied areas.
Speaker 1 (52:10):
So consider populations of individuals that have had visually overwhelming
traumatic experiences. So many traumatic experiences are necessarily visual, but
car accident war veterans where the visual input just floods
the memories night mirrors, and they're constantly flooded and distracted.
Speaker 2 (52:30):
To no end by these visuals.
Speaker 1 (52:32):
So there's an area in the kind of the back
of the brain that my ex colleague Steve Costlin spent
decades uncovering, which is the visual imagery area.
Speaker 2 (52:43):
So you invite individuals with.
Speaker 1 (52:45):
These kind of visually traumatic experiences to come in, and
you invite them to bring back those images, and while
they're doing it, you are stimulating the visual imagery area incredible,
And what you find is you basically fade the visual
imagery component without losing the memory of the experience, so
(53:06):
that now when it comes back, they're just not flooded
as much by those images.
Speaker 3 (53:11):
That's like a top. Now that's what I was talking
about earlier, exactly right. It seems more hopeful to me,
It's much more hopeful.
Speaker 1 (53:17):
So you also don't get the galvanic skin response because
it's not causing that response. So in sometimes yes, the
body keeps the score, but it's coming out. The memory
keeps the score, but the body is showing some of
the outputs.
Speaker 3 (53:29):
Well that's what. Yeah. In another way, Lisa Feldman Barrett says,
the body is the scorecard. Yes, the brain keeps the
score and the bodies of the.
Speaker 2 (53:39):
Score card exactly right.
Speaker 1 (53:41):
The ledger CONTs similar TOIL, very similar.
Speaker 2 (53:46):
So that's one.
Speaker 1 (53:47):
The second, of course, which is, you know, become part
of the popular press. But I think it's I cover
a lot more of the science in the book. Is
the sort of psychedelics revolution. Oh yeah, And I think
the key thing that kind of note here for your listeners,
of course, is that it's not let's just go have
an LSD trip. It's therapy assisted psychedelic use.
Speaker 2 (54:11):
It's not microdosing.
Speaker 1 (54:12):
It's therapy assisted of a particular kind of therapy. And
to me, what I love about the science is that
it's beautiful science. And it's beautiful to me in the
following way. So what they do are they called doubly
blind experiments, So neither the patient nor the therapists know.
Speaker 2 (54:34):
Who got the placebo and who got the psychedelic nice. Okay.
Speaker 1 (54:39):
The second reason why it's beautiful science is because we're
beginning to really understand the mechanism of how the psychedelics.
Speaker 2 (54:46):
Are affecting the brain.
Speaker 1 (54:47):
Yes, okay, so in the case of major depression, it's
psilocybin mountic mushrooms. In the case of PTSD, often complex PTSD,
it's MDMA. So let me talk about the MDMA one
because I think I really like this hopefulness of this
result in both the depression case and the in the
PTSD case, they're picking individual populations that are what are
(55:12):
called treatment resistant, meaning they've gone for at least a
decade where neither medication nor therapy has had any impact.
Speaker 3 (55:21):
Let's be really frustrating, really frustrating.
Speaker 2 (55:23):
So these are people at their end their rope right there.
Speaker 1 (55:24):
Of course, they're just suffering all the horrible, horrible.
Speaker 3 (55:28):
People like that. Let's get them some help.
Speaker 2 (55:32):
Let's get themselves.
Speaker 1 (55:33):
Eight weeks of md M A and therapy knocks the
PTSD diagnosis by down by seventy percent. That's why they're
very close now to FDA approval, because they've gone through
the key kind of criteria for getting FDA prod.
Speaker 3 (55:50):
This is super interesting. MDMA obviously operates in a different mechanism.
It's a different system than than psilocybin, for instance, and
so they both have their own mechanisms of action. Correct.
Do you think what the it's like It has to
do with serotonin sort of situation going on there.
Speaker 1 (56:07):
Yeah, serotonin and potentially oxytocin, and this comes.
Speaker 2 (56:10):
From animal work.
Speaker 1 (56:11):
So this is a really really cool result. So this
is a guy named Roman Nardeau who's a kind of
neuroscientist who's actually used MDMA with mice.
Speaker 2 (56:21):
This is no therapy.
Speaker 3 (56:22):
Do they start hugging each other and dancing?
Speaker 1 (56:25):
Yes, here's the really cool thing. You reopen the critical
period for social interactions in mice with MDMA and oxytocin.
Speaker 3 (56:36):
That's incredible.
Speaker 2 (56:37):
So this is.
Speaker 1 (56:38):
Where the combination of work with animals and humans is
really beginning to shine the light on what's going on mechanistically,
and that opening of the critical period is the key part.
Is of course, people who take MDMA ecstasy parties.
Speaker 2 (56:50):
It's because of the social.
Speaker 1 (56:51):
Stuff and that's what's it's opening up as sociality, which
of course and critically is opening it up to therapy
in the same way that silos in depression major depression,
the default network which is kind of the autobiographical piece
is taking the first position in our thinking. For depressed people,
(57:13):
that's why I can't get out of their own way.
It's like, why am I always so sorry? Why am
I always so sad?
Speaker 3 (57:18):
Right?
Speaker 2 (57:19):
What psilocybin does is it quiets that system down and
opens up other networks, like the salience network, making the
individual more open to therapy.
Speaker 3 (57:29):
It self is such a source of our suffering, isn't it.
Speaker 1 (57:32):
You'd rather be an Apleasia. I'd rather be an Apleasia,
no self.
Speaker 3 (57:37):
Self, That's yeah. I mean when you think of the
you know, just our self narratives and how that kind
of creates wars, I'm not just internal strife, but it
explains a lot of strife around the world. But I
also think the default network is really good for creativity though,
so I've done. I've published studies showing it's not all bad.
It's all I call it the imagination network totally. That's right. Hey,
(58:01):
this was such an elucidating conversation and so important. I
can tell that you are. I'm glad I met you.
You know you're not the villain that you've been made
out to be. At a certain point in my life
when you were made out to be. It's such a
I'm glad I met you. You are, You're obviously so compassionate,
and you you get authentically emotional when you talk about
helping these kids. I am sitting right next to you,
(58:22):
and I see it and it's so authentic, and it's
just it's beautiful. Thank you, so thank you for everything
you're doing.
Speaker 2 (58:28):
Yeah, thank you for the conversation. It's been awesome.
Speaker 3 (58:30):
Yeah,