Episode Transcript
Available transcripts are automatically generated. Complete accuracy is not guaranteed.
(00:00):
Welcome back to the Deep Dive. Today we're really getting into
something fascinating. Malcolm Gladwell's book Blink.
The Power of Thinking without Thinking.
Yeah, it's all about those snap judgments, those instant
feelings we get. Exactly.
And our mission, you know, is tounpack all the surprising stuff
from the book, why these blinks happen when they're right, when
they're wrong, and maybe even how to get better at them all,
(00:22):
with hopefully a bit of fun along the way.
It's definitely a trip into how our minds work.
Often behind the scenes, these quick takes, they're, they're
everywhere, shaping so much morethan we probably realize.
Yeah. You know who we trust to what
art we like. Totally.
And to kick things off, let's jump right into this amazing
story. Gladwell uses the mystery of the
(00:43):
Getty Choros. So picture this.
It's the 80's. The Jay Paul Getty Museum is
about to drop millions on this supposedly ancient Greek statue,
a chorus. This like perfect young man
carved in marble, meant to be from way back like 530 BC.
And they weren't messing around.This wasn't some impulse buy.
They spent months, maybe even longer, doing all the science.
(01:05):
Geologists looked at the marble chemists.
Check the surface the the patina.
What that aged look? Exactly.
Conservators examined every little detail.
Lawyers traced its history, trying to figure out where it
had been, who owned it, Everything.
All the hard data was pointing to one thing.
Authentic. Priceless.
Yeah, the science team, the legal team, everyone was thumbs
(01:26):
up. They had stacks of evidence,
confident. But then then comes the twist.
They decide to show it off, basically get the final blessing
from the real heavy hitters, thetop sculpture experts in the
world over in Athens. And these experts, guys who've
spent their entire lives lookingat ancient sculptures, they walk
in, take one look, and boom, thereactions are instant, totally
(01:48):
gut level, and completely the opposite of all that careful
research. It's wild, isn't it?
Like Thomas Hoving, former head of the Met in New York.
He talks about this immediate intuitive repulsion just felt
wrong. Famously told them get your
money back just like that. Yeah, and George Despinus, head
of the Acropolis Museum, he apparently went pale and just
(02:08):
said anyone who has ever seen a sculpture coming out of the
ground could tell that that thing has never been in the
ground. Think about that phrasing.
Never been in the ground. It's not a scientific term.
It's pure expert observation, pure feeling based on deep
experience. And Angeles Delirious, another
museum director. He talked about stylistic
(02:29):
issues, sure, but also this feeling, he said.
It was like there was a glass between me and the work.
Wow, a glass like a barrier to authenticity he could just
sense. Right, so here you have these
top experts. Their whole reaction is based on
this, this blink, this feeling, and it completely contradicts
months of science and legal work.
(02:51):
So the big question becomes how?How does that split second
feeling trump all the data? And who was right in the end?
Can you actually trust that gut instinct?
And that's the core of Blink, really.
Spoiler alert for the Corros, bythe way.
Yeah, those experts, they were totally right.
The Getty's case just crumbled, the documents proving its
history forged. They had like postal codes and
(03:13):
bank accounts that didn't even exist back when the letters were
supposedly written. Oh wow, classic forgery mistake.
Pain the science. Turns out you can fake that
ancient patina. Apparently a bit of potato mold.
Yeah, potato mold on the right kind of marble can age it
artificially in just a couple ofmonths.
Potato mold. You can't make this stuff up.
(03:35):
So the multi $1,000,000 statue was a fake.
A very sophisticated one, but still fake.
Exactly. Which is just this incredible
illustration of expert intuition, right?
So our mission today is to really dig into that, guided by
Gladwell. Why are these instant takes so
powerful? When do they work?
When do they totally fail us? And crucially, can we actually
train ourselves to have better blends?
(03:56):
Links. We're going to look at some
really surprising stuff in the book, get into the nitty gritty
of how our minds make these leaps.
It's going to be interesting. OK, so let's dive in.
Let's talk about the unconsciousbrain, this sort of silent
partner we all have. All right, let's start with this
really cool experiment that kindof lifts the veil on how this
unconscious stuff works. It shows our brains knowing
(04:18):
things way before we consciouslyknow them.
It's the Iowa gambling experiment.
Yes, a classic. So imagine you're in the study.
You sit down. There are 4 decks of cards, 2
red, 2 blue. Simple task.
Picks cards win money, some cards give you cash, some hit
you with penalties. Goal is just to maximize your
winnings. But here's the secret setup
(04:40):
right? The decks aren't equal.
The participants don't know this, but two decks are, let's
say, good. The blue ones maybe they give
you smaller wins like 50 bucks, but the penalties are also
small, maybe 10 bucks. Play these, you'll slowly make
money. Then there are the bad decks,
the red ones. They tempt you with big wins,
maybe $100. But lurking in there are these
huge, devastating penalties. Hundreds of dollars sometimes.
(05:03):
Keep picking from the red decks and you're guaranteed to go
broke eventually. Now this is where it gets really
fascinating. They hook the gamblers up to
machines measuring sweat gland activity in their palms.
You're that slightly clammy feeling when you're nervous.
Yeah, like when you're about to ask for a raise or something.
Exactly. They measured that, and what
they found was kind of spooky. People started showing physical
(05:25):
stress responses, sweaty palms when they just hovered their
hand over the bad red decks, andthis started happening
incredibly early, like around the 10th card they picked. 10th
card Their bodies were already sending out warning flares like
Danger Will Robinson. But here's the kicker, the real
mind Bender. They developed this physical
(05:45):
stress response, this sweaty palm thing, and actually started
subconsciously favoring the goodblue decks. 40 cards before they
could consciously explain why they couldn't say oh the red
decks are rigged. Not yet. 40 cards earlier, so
their unconscious mind had already crunched the numbers,
figured out the pattern of risk and reward, and sent out the
warning via sweaty palms way before the conscious brain
(06:09):
caught up. It's amazing.
It's like your brain has these two systems running in parallel.
The slow, deliberate, conscious one that needs lots of data,
thinks things through, and eventually figures out the game
after maybe 80 cards, right? The logical one and then?
And there's this over super fast, totally unconscious
operating, like Gladwell says, below the surface of
consciousness. And it communicates indirectly
(06:31):
through gut feelings, sweaty palms, maybe a little flutter in
your stomach. It guides your behavior even
when you have no conscious clue why you're suddenly avoiding the
red decks. It's like having an internal
early warning system that speaksa different language.
Not words, but feelings and physical responses.
What's really cool here is how it shows the brain prioritizing
(06:51):
speed and efficiency, especiallywhen things are moving fast,
where the stakes feel high. It processes complex patterns
and gives you these nudges, these feelings, without
bothering your conscious mind with all the details.
It's like your intuition is literally your body talking to
you. OK, so that's gambling.
Now let's switch gears to something maybe even more
complex. Human relationships.
(07:13):
This brings us to John Gottman and his incredible Love Lab.
Gottman Yeah, his work is legendary.
Seriously think about this. Predicting whether a married
couple will stay together or getdivorced with really high
accuracy just by watching them argue for like 15 minutes.
It sounds like fortune telling. It does, but Gottman's been
(07:34):
doing it for decades, with startling success.
He brings couples into his lab, videotapes them talking about
some point of conflict that, youknow, the usual stuff, money,
chores, whatever. That everyday friction.
Exactly. And then he applies this
incredibly detailed coding system called Spell AFF Specific
Effect. They categorize every single
emotional expression second by second.
(07:55):
We're talking disgust, contempt,anger, sadness, defensiveness,
stonewalling, whining like 20 different categories.
Wow, second by second. That sounds unbelievably
painstaking, like that being thehuman emotional genome or
something. It's intensely data heavy.
A 15 minute check could generatelike 1800 data points plus heart
rate, skin temperature. You'd think you need a
(08:17):
supercomputer to make sense of it all.
You absolutely would. How could you possibly boil that
down? But then Gottman had this
brilliant insight and analogy that cut through the complexity.
He compared it to military fists.
Fists like punching. No, no, like in Morse code.
During World War 2, radio operators sending Morse code
each had a unique unconscious rhythm and touched their fist.
(08:40):
British intelligence got so goodthey could identify individual
German operators just by their fist.
Track units across Europe based on who is transmitting.
It's like a fingerprint, an automatic signature.
OK, so it's not something they tried to do, it just happened
naturally. Exactly.
It's inherent, and Gottman realized relationships have a
fist to a core underlying pattern of interaction.
(09:01):
He discovered he didn't need all1800 data points.
He could 0 in on the most predictive patterns, especially
what he famously called the FourHorsemen of the Apocalypse for
relationships. Right, I've heard of those
defensiveness, stonewalling, criticism and the really bad
one. Contempt.
Yes, contempt in Gottmann research is the absolute kiss of
(09:22):
death. It's more than just anger or
criticism. It's looking down on your
partner, treating them like they're beneath you.
It's like infused with disgust. Exactly.
It's hierarchical. Think eye rolling, sneering,
mockery, hostile humor, name calling.
It's saying you're worthless. That's incredibly corrosive
because it attacks the person's sense of self, their value
within the relationship. And didn't he find that actually
(09:45):
effects physical health too? Astonishingly, yes.
The consistent presence of contempt in a marriage is such a
powerful stressor that it can actually predict a spouse's
susceptibility to illnesses likecolds and flu.
It seems to hammer the immune system.
That's how profound the connection between emotional
state and physical well-being is.
That is genuinely scary, wow. And Gottman got so good at
(10:06):
reading these fists, at this kind of thin slicing of
marriages, he claimed he could overhear a couple arguing at the
next table in a restaurant and get a pretty accurate sense of
whether they were headed for divorce court.
That's incredible. It's like developing
relationship X-ray vision. It really shows how expertise
sharpens intuition. Through all that intense
analysis, he trained his unconscious mind to instantly
(10:29):
pick out the most critical signals, the contempt, the
defensiveness from all the surrounding noise.
So, OK, putting these together, the Iowa gamblers, Gottman's
love Lab, it's clear our unconscious mind is this amazing
processor making sophisticated judgments super fast, often
without us even knowing how or why.
It's just humming away back there.
(10:49):
Definitely a powerful silent partner, but not always a
reliable one. Right, because as amazing as
this unconscious stuff is, it's definitely not perfect.
These blinks, these instant impressions, can also mess us up
badly. Sometimes.
They totally deceive us. And this, for me, is where it
gets really interesting and maybe a bit unsettling too.
For sure. And one of the biggest pitfalls
(11:10):
is something Gladwell calls the storytelling problem.
Basically, our conscious mind islike a compulsive storyteller.
It needs to have a reason for why we do things or feel things.
And if the real reason is hiddenin the unconscious, the
conscious mind just, well, it makes up a story.
A plausible one, usually, but often completely wrong.
(11:32):
OK, explain that more. How does that work?
Gladwell uses John Berg's priming experiments.
Fantastic stuff. Imagine you're a student
volunteer. You're given lists of words,
maybe 5 words, and you have to make a grammatical forward
sentence. Seems like a simple language
task. OK, sounds harmless enough.
But here's the trick. Hidden in those word lists are
words subtly related to, say, old age words like worried,
(11:56):
Florida, lonely, Gray, bingo wrinkle.
The students aren't consciously noticing a theme, they're just
making sentences. This is called priming.
So their unconscious is getting nudged toward the concept of old
age. Exactly, and the mind blowing
result after they finish the test and leave the room.
These students who've been unconsciously primed with old
(12:17):
age words literally walk more slowly down the hallway than
students who got neutral words. No way.
Seriously, just from reading words like wrinkle?
Seriously, their bodies just sort of acted old without them
having any conscious awareness of why they slowed down.
If you asked them, they'd say, oh, I was just thinking or no
reason. They wouldn't say because I was
(12:38):
primed with geriatric keywords. That's wild.
So our behavior can be influenced by stuff we don't
even notice. Absolutely.
They did it with other primes too.
Prime students with aggressive words?
They're more likely to interruptthe experimenter later.
Prime them with polite words, they wait patiently.
It shows how easily our unconscious can be steered by
subtle cues. OK, so how does this link back
(13:00):
to the storytelling problem? We'll think about Mayor's two
rope problem. Classic psych experiment.
You're in a room with two ropes hanging down too far apart to
grab both at once. There are tools around,
including pliers. Most people get stuck.
Yeah, I think I've heard of thisone.
Right, so after a while, the experimenter might casually walk
by and accidentally brush one rope, maybe with the pliers tied
(13:21):
to it, making it swing a little.Often.
Soon after that, the subject gets the aha moment.
Tie the pliers to one rope, swing it like a pendulum, catch
it while holding the other rope.Problem solved.
OK, makes sense. The hint worked.
But here's the storytelling part.
When you ask them how they solved it, they almost never
say, oh, when you brush the rope.
(13:42):
It gave me the idea. They'll say things like it just
came to me, or I thought about how Tarzan swings on vines, or I
realized the pliers had weight. They invent A logical, conscious
reason for a solution that was actually sparked by an
unconscious cue. So their conscious brain creates
a neat, tidy story to explain the unconsciously.
Precisely. It's like our conscious mind is
(14:04):
the press secretary for our unconscious president, always
ready with a plausible explanation, even if it's not
the real 1. Gladwell even brings up one of
the OJ Simpson jurors who insisted on TV that Ray's had
absolutely nothing to do with her verdict.
But how could she possibly know that for sure?
Given what we know about primingan unconscious bias, it's highly
(14:24):
likely it played some role, evenif she genuinely believed it
didn't. We're often ignorant of what
truly influences us, but we rarely feel ignorant.
We just tell ourselves a story. That's a bit disturbing
actually, if we can't even trustour own explanations for our
actions. It definitely complicates
things, and it raises that big question, if our minds are doing
this, how reliable are our firstimpressions of other people?
(14:47):
Which leads us perfectly to the classic Warren Harding error.
President Harding not exactly remembered as a great leader, is
he? No.
Widely considered one of the worst, actually.
But the fascinating thing is howhe became president.
It was largely because he just looked the part.
Tall, handsome, dignified, silver hair, deep voice.
He looked presidential. He had the look.
(15:08):
Exactly. His campaign manager, Harry
Doherty, basically saw him and thought, wow, that guy looks
like a president. It was an instant visual
judgement, a blink. But the reality behind the look
was less impressive. Much less impressive.
By all accounts, he wasn't very bright, preferred poker, golf
and chasing women to actual governing, was vague on major
(15:29):
issues, often skipped important debates.
The image and the substance weremiles apart.
So the Warren Harding error is basically falling for the
packaging, judging someone's ability, especially leadership,
based purely on their appearance, particularly that
tall commanding presence. That's the essence of it.
We have this automatic unconscious bias associating
(15:50):
physical stature and conventional good looks in men
with competence and leadership ability.
Our blink gets hijacked by the visual stereotype.
And there's actual data backing this up, isn't there?
It's not just Harding. Oh yeah, the data's pretty
stark. Gladwell cites studies showing
male CEO's and Fortune 500 companies are on average,
significantly taller than the general population, something
like 58% or 6 feet or taller, compared to only about 14.5% of
(16:15):
American men overall. Wow.
That's a huge difference. It is, and it translates to
salary too. Other research suggests every
inch of height is worth hundreds, maybe even close to
$800 a year extra in salary overa career.
That's a massive advantage purely based on how tall you
are. It's a clear, measurable
unconscious bias. We just seem to defer to tole
(16:36):
men. Which ties right into the whole
idea of hidden biases like the ones revealed by the Implicit
Association test, the IAT. Exactly.
The IAT is that computer test that measures our unconscious
attitudes. It pairs concepts like good and
bad with categories like white faces and black faces, and
measures how quickly we make theassociations.
And the results are often kind of uncomfortable, right?
(16:57):
Even for people who consciously believe in equality.
They can be very uncomfortable. The test often reveals that most
people, regardless of their own race or explicit beliefs, have
faster, stronger unconscious associations linking, say, white
with good than black with good. This isn't necessarily conscious
prejudice. It reflects the deeply ingrained
(17:17):
cultural messages and stereotypes we absorb without
even realizing it. So my conscious self might be
totally egalitarian, but my automatic unconscious blink
might still carry those biases learned from society.
That's what the IAT suggests, yes, and it reinforces the
Warren Harding error. These automatic associations,
whether it's heightened leadership or race and positive
(17:39):
traits, can really skew our judgments in ways we're not
aware of. We think we're being objective,
but our unconscious blank is playing by different rules.
OK, so this is sounding a bit bleak.
Are we just doomed to be puppetsof our unconscious biases?
Well, Gladwell offers a counterpoint, a story of someone
who actively fought against thisBob Gollum, the incredibly
successful car salesman. The car salesman, how does he
(18:00):
fit in? Gullum operated by a simple,
almost radical rule for his lineof work.
You cannot prejudge people in this business.
He actively resisted the urge tomake snap judgements based on
appearance. So the opposite of the Warren
Harding error. Exactly.
He told stories about guys and dirty overalls looking like they
couldn't afford a hubcap who'd walk in and pay cash for the
(18:23):
most expensive car on the lot, or teenagers just browsing who
seemed clueless but came back later with their parents ready
to buy. He learned that prejudging was,
as he put it, the kiss of death.So he trained himself not to
trust his initial blink about a customer's potential.
Precisely. His success came from
deliberately treating everyone the same, giving everyone his
(18:45):
full attention and respect regardless of how they looked or
dressed. He consciously overrode that
natural tendency to thin slice based on appearance.
It shows that while these biasesare powerful, they are destiny.
We can recognize them and implement strategies to
counteract them. OK, that's a bit more hopeful.
It means we can exercise some control.
We can. It requires awareness and
(19:06):
conscious effort, but it's possible.
Gallone story shows that understanding the potential
flaws in our blink is the first step towards making better
judgments. So wrapping the section up, it's
clear our unconscious is this double edged sword.
Incredibly powerful but also prone to biases, easily
manipulated by subtle cues, and sometimes just plain wrong,
(19:27):
especially when we try to explain it after the fact.
It's a tool we need to understand deeply, warts and
all. OK, so we've seen the power of
the blink, and also how it can lead us astray with biases and
storytelling. But here's another angle from
the book that kind of flips the script.
Sometimes having too much information is actually the
enemy. It can paralyze us, overwhelm
our intuition. Yeah, this is a really
(19:48):
counterintuitive idea, isn't it?We usually think more data
equals better decisions, but Gladwell argues that's often not
the case, especially for decisions that rely on rapid
cognition on that expert blink. He uses the example of Cook
County Hospital's emergency department, which sounds like
well controlled chaos, maybe especially with potential heart
attack patients. It's exactly huge pressure, lots
(20:11):
of uncertainty, doctors drowningin information, patient history,
symptoms, lifestyle factors, ECGreadings, trying to make
critical decisions fast but often getting bogged down.
Beds were full. Diagnosis were slow.
A classic case of information overload leading to bottlenecks.
Right. Then along comes Doctor Brennan
Riley, and he decides to try an algorithm developed by a
(20:32):
cardiologist named Lee Goldman. And this algorithm was radical
because it was so incredibly simple.
Simple how? What did it cut?
Out It basically ignored almost everything doctors normally
considered forget age, sex, smoking history, stress levels,
past surgeries, all that extra detail.
The algorithm focused only on the ECG reading plus just three
key risk factors. Is the patient pain?
(20:53):
Unstable angina? Is there fluid in the lungs and
is there systolic blood pressurebelow 100?
That's it. Just three things, plus the ECG.
That sounds risky, like you're throwing away potentially vital
information. That's what you'd think.
But here's the kicker. The simple algorithm was
dramatically better than the doctor's judgement.
It was 70% more accurate at identifying patients who weren't
(21:15):
actually having a heart attack, freeing up beds.
And it was significantly better,like 95% accuracy versus 7589%
for the doctors at spotting the patients who did need urgent
care. Wow, so less information led to
more accuracy? How is that possible?
Gladwell's point is that all that extra information wasn't
(21:35):
just useless for the immediate critical diagnosis, it was
actually harmful. It confused the issues.
It muddied the waters, overloaded the doctor's
cognitive capacity, and actuallyimpaired their judgement.
In that high pressure moment, the algorithm worked because it
was frugal, focusing only on thetruly decisive variables.
It's like the signal got lost inthe noise and the algorithm just
cut out the noise. Precisely, it forced a focus on
(21:58):
what really mattered for that initial rapid assessment.
It shows that sometimes our blink works best when it's not
cluttered with excessive detail.OK, that's medicine.
But this applies elsewhere too, right?
Like war games? Oh yeah, the Millennium
Challenge story is another fantastic illustration.
This was this massive, super expensive war game the Pentagon
ran in 2002. Blue team was the US armed with
(22:18):
all the latest tech networked information systems,
overwhelming data analysis basically designed to prove that
information superiority wins wars through systematic
planning. So, the ultimate data-driven
approach to warfare. Exactly.
But then they cast retired Marine Lieutenant General Paul
Van Ripper as the commander of Red Team, the enemy.
And Van Ripper was, well, he wasa believer in the fog of war.
(22:42):
He thought war was inherently messy, unpredictable, chaotic,
The total opposite of Blue Team's philosophy.
A clash of styles, then the systematic planners versus the
intuitive chaos agent. Totally, and being right for
strategy for Red team was deliberately low tech and
unpredictable. No fancy encrypted comms that
could be hacked. He used motorcycle couriers,
messages hidden in calls to prayer, stuff Blue team wasn't
(23:03):
even looking for. Old school.
Very old school and instead of reacting defensively, he went on
the offensive immediately. He launched a massive pre
emptive missile strike calculated to overwhelm Blue
Team's defenses. He clogged the Persian Gulf with
forms of small cheap boats, too many for Blue Team sophisticated
sensors to track effectively. He encouraged the commanders to
(23:24):
act on instinct, make snap decisions.
He created chaos basically. He absolutely did.
Meanwhile, Blue Team was, as Gladwell puts it, gorging on
information. They had this immense database,
constant analysis, complex models, but their system
required them to stop, analyze, discuss, get approval.
It forced deliberation. Which is fine if you have time
(23:46):
and the enemy plays by the rules.
Right, but Van Ripper wasn't giving them a predictable logic
problem. He was giving them an insight
problem, something that requiredquick, intuitive adaptation.
Gladwell uses the analogy of a firefighter who smells smoke,
sees a small kitchen fire, but then feels heat under the
floorboards in the living room and instantly knows the real
danger is hidden below, orderingeveryone out without needing to
(24:07):
analyze combustion rates. It's a blink based on deep
experience. And Blue team, with all their
data, couldn't make that kind ofintuitive leap.
They were paralyzed. Their system wasn't built for
it. They couldn't factor Van
Ripper's unpredictable intuitivemoves into their logical
equations. Their focus on systematic
analysis had, in effect, extinguish their ability to
(24:29):
react intuitively. Red Team, relying on simpler
methods and rapid cognition, absolutely crushed them in the
initial phase of the war game. So again, too much information,
too much analysis actually hampered their ability to make
effective, timely decisions in afluid saturation.
It's a powerful lesson in situations that demand fast
(24:49):
adaptation and insight. Information overload can be
crippling. Which actually brings us back to
something much more mundane. Jam Jam.
How does jam fit into war games and heart attacks?
Remember Sheena Iyengar's jam experiment, The one where she
set up tasting booths in a fancygrocery store?
Oh right, with the different numbers of jam.
Exactly. Some days she offered 24
different varieties of gourmet jam, other days just six.
(25:13):
Now, common sense says more choices, better, right?
Offered 24 jams. People are more likely to find
one they love and buy it. Yeah, you'd think that for sure.
More options, more sales. But the results were the
complete opposite. When faced with 24 jams, lots of
people stopped to taste, but only 3% actually bought any jam,
3%. Wow.
That's tiny, but on the days with only six jams, far fewer
(25:35):
choices. But 30% of the people who
stopped actually bought jam 10 times the conversion rate.
So offering less choice led to way more sales.
Significantly more. The take away, Gladwell
suggests, is that choosing jam is usually a snap decision, a
blink. It's not something we agonize
over. When you present too many
options for a simple choice likethat, it overwhelms our ability
(25:57):
to make that quick, intuitive judgement.
We get paralyzed by the analysis.
Our blank mechanism short circuits.
Analysis paralysis, even for jam.
Exactly. Snap judgments are frugal, the
book says. They work best with limited
relevant information. Trying to process too much
detail for a simple decision actually makes the decision
harder, not easier. So connecting all this Cook
(26:18):
County Millennium Challenge the jet, it seems like our intuitive
mind has the sweet spot for information enough to make a
good call, but not so much that it gets bogged down.
That seems to be the key insight.
Knowing how much information is just enough is crucial for
effective, rapid cognition. Sometimes dripping away the
excess is the most important step towards clarity.
(26:40):
OK, let's shift focus a bit. We've talked about how the
amount of information matters, but what about the context
surrounding our blinks? Does the environment, the
presentation, how we encounter something affect our snap
judgments? Hugely.
This is another really critical theme in Blink.
Our rapid cognition doesn't happen in a vacuum.
It's incredibly sensitive to context, sometimes in ways that
(27:01):
lead us astray if the context isartificial or misleading.
Gladwell uses the example of this musician Kenna.
Tell us about him. Kenna is this really interesting
case. Music industry insiders like
record execs, producers, even U2's manager Paul McGuinness,
people with deep expertise. They heard Kenna and instantly
(27:21):
got it. They loved his music, size,
potential, recognizes talent almost immediately.
A classic positive blink from the experts.
So the people who really know music heard him and thought,
yes, this is it. Right.
But here's the problem. When his music was tested on the
mass audience through typical market research methods.
Playing snippets over the phone,online surveys, that kind of
(27:43):
thing. The reaction was lukewarm,
mostly negative. Actually.
The general public didn't get him.
That's weird. Why the huge disconnect between
the experts and the audience? The argument is context.
The experts who love Kenna experience his music in a rich
context. They maybe saw him perform live,
felt the energy of the crowd, heard the full power of his
voice in person, understood his artistic background.
(28:04):
Their blink was informed by all the surrounding information.
OK, they got the full picture. But the market research that was
decontextualized, thin slicing, just a snippet of sound, maybe
compressed over a phone line with none of the atmosphere, the
visual cues, the story, the audience's blink was based on
incomplete, sterile data. And it LED them to a different,
(28:25):
less positive conclusion. It's like judging a meal based
only on smelling 1 ingredient blindfolded.
That's a great analogy, and it connects perfectly to another
famous marketing story, the New Coke disaster.
New Coke the cautionary tale. Absolutely.
Coca-Cola spent millions on blind taste tests.
They pitted their new formula against Pepsi and also against
(28:47):
original Coke. And in these tests, New Coke
consistently won. People preferred the taste when
they didn't know what they were drinking.
The data was clear, right Blind taste tests showed New Coke was
the winner. Overwhelmingly clear by
significant margins, Coke executives were ecstatic.
They thought they had a guaranteed hit, calling it the
surest move the company's ever made.
But then they launched it and itwas a total catastrophe.
(29:09):
Monumental disaster, Consumer outrage, protests, hoarding of
old coke. They were forced to backtrack
and bring back classic Coke justa few months later.
So what went wrong? The taste test said people liked
it better. The problem was the test itself,
the context. First, a SIP test, what they
call a Central location test or CLT, is very different from
(29:31):
drinking a whole can or bottle of soda over time.
Sips tend to favor sweeter formulas, which New Coke was.
Home use tests where people drink it normally give much
better data on overall preference.
OK, so the test method was flawed.
That was part of it. But the bigger mistake, the core
contextual error, was ignoring the brand.
People don't drink Coca-Cola blind.
(29:52):
They drink it out of that iconicred can.
They see the logo? They have decades of
associations, memories, feelingstied up with the idea of Coke.
The brand itself has has a powerful unconscious meaning.
Hugely powerful. The taste test stripped away all
that meaning, all that context and focus only on the raw
sensory data of taste. But in the real world, people
(30:12):
consume the brand, the image, the feeling just as much as the
liquid itself. Coke forgot that their product
wasn't just sugar water, it was a cultural icon.
So the blink in the taste test was misleading because it lacked
the crucial context of the brandidentity.
Exactly, and this power of packaging and presentation to
influence our perception, our unconscious blink, is
(30:34):
everywhere. Gladwell mentions other examples
margarine tasting better when itwas yellow, had a crown on the
package and came in foil wrap even though it was identical to
plain white margarine. Or adding a tiny sprig of
parsley to the label of canned meat to make it seem fresher.
Yes, or putting tomato sauce in a rustic looking glass jar to
evoke grandmothers cooking. Or how ice cream apparently
(30:57):
tastes better from a cylindricalcontainer than a rectangular 1,
even if the ice cream is exactlythe same.
It's all about manipulating those unconscious associations
through context and presentation.
Which raises an ethical questionmaybe.
It certainly does. Is it manipulation or just
skillfully understanding and catering to human perception?
Either way, it shows how powerfully context shapes our
(31:19):
experience, often without us realizing it.
OK. Another example related to this
idea of initial impressions versus actual experience is the
Aaron Chair. Yes, the famous Herman Miller
chair. Revolutionary design, but
definitely unconventional looking when it first came out.
Understatement. Gladwell describes it as looking
like the exoskeleton of a giant prehistoric insect.
(31:41):
Black plastic mesh, weird curves.
Not your typical comfy office chair look.
Not at all and when they did early user testing people
actually rated it low on comfortlike 4.75 out of 10.
They look at this wiry, transparent thing and just
couldn't believe it would be comfortable or even hold them up
properly. So their initial visual blink
screamed uncomfortable, even if that wasn't the reality.
(32:02):
Exactly. The book suggests people,
especially Americans back then, were conditioned by big, plush
LA Z boy style chairs. They associated comfort with
bulk and softness. The Aaron's minimalist, high
tech aesthetic was completely counterintuitive to their
preconceived notions of comfort.It didn't look comfortable
according to their mental template.
(32:23):
Right. But here's the interesting part.
As people actually use the chairfor a longer period SAT and it
worked in it, their perception completely shifted.
Comfort scores soared up to 8 orhigher.
The actual experience eventuallyoverrode the misleading initial
aesthetic blink. So experience corrected the
flawed first impression, but it took time.
(32:43):
It did, and initially facility managers, the people who buy
office furniture, hated it too. They called it lawn furniture or
Robocop's chair. The aesthetic blink was
incredibly powerful, almost blinding people to the chairs
actual ergonomic benefits until familiarity and positive
experience gradually changed minds.
Which brings us back, interestingly, to Jam and the
idea of explaining our blinks. Yes, the jam experiment
(33:07):
revisited. Remember, experts and regular
students initially agreed prettywell on which jams were best
just by tasting them. Right, their intuitive rankings
were similar. But then they were in a
variation. They asked a different group of
students not just to rank the jams, but to write down their
reasons for liking or disliking each one.
Explain your choice. OK, seems reasonable.
(33:29):
Analyze your preferences. But the result was disastrous.
Asking them to verbalize their reasons completely scramble
their judgement. Their rankings became useless,
totally out of whack with the experts.
They even ranks the top rated jam Knott's Berry Farm near the
bottom. Just because they had to explain
why they liked it? How does that work?
Gladwell's explanation is that we often lack the precise
(33:51):
vocabulary, especially for something like taste, to
accurately describe our unconscious preferences.
When forced to explain, we startreaching for plausible sounding
reasons. It's fruity.
It's tangy, it's not too sweet. We invent A narrative, and the
act of inventing that conscious narrative actually overrides or
interferes with our true intuitive preference.
(34:13):
We essentially talk ourselves out of our own gut feeling.
We become jam idiots, as the book puts it.
Exactly. It's the storytelling problem
again, but in reverse. Trying to consciously analyze
and explain an unconscious feeling can actually corrupt the
feeling itself, especially when the underlying drivers are
complex and hard to articulate. So pulling this all together
(34:33):
kind of new cope, the errand share the jam explanation.
It really hammers home that our blinks are incredibly sensitive
to context. Taking something out of its
natural context, like in a blindtaste test, can give you a
misleading result, and forcing conscious explanation onto an
unconscious judgement can wreck it entirely.
It means a blink. Blink is often only as good as
the context it's made in. Understanding and managing that
(34:56):
context is just as important as understanding the blink itself.
OK, so we've seen how powerful blinks are, how they can be
biased, affected by information overload, and highly dependent
on context. Now let's look at what happens
when these snap judgments go tragically wrong in really high
stakes situations, and, crucially, what we can learn
about mastering our instincts. This inevitably brings us back
(35:19):
to the incredibly sad and complex story of Amadou Diallo,
the unarmed man shot 41 times byNew York City police officers in
1999. It serves as a stark example of
a catastrophic failure and rapidcognition, specifically in what
Gladwell calls mind reading. Mind reading meaning like
instantly assessing someone's intentions in emotional state.
(35:41):
Exactly. It's that fundamental, usually
unconscious process of figuring out what's going on in someone
else's head based on their expressions, body language, the
situation. We do it constantly.
The officers involved in the Diala shooting saw him reach
into his pocket late at night ina high crime area vestibule.
And their blink interpretation was threat gun right when?
And reality was innocent man, possibly confused or scared,
(36:03):
reaching for his wallet. That split second
misinterpretation, that failure of mind, reading under immense
pressure, had devastating consequences.
The book kind of pushes back against the simple explanations,
either pure accident or pure racism.
It does Gladwell suggest a middle ground, acknowledging
that while bias might play a role, the core issue in that
(36:24):
moment was likely a complex failure of this rapid, intuitive
mind reading process, amplified by stress in the specific
circumstances. These failures, he argues, are
surprisingly common, though usually with far less tragic
outcomes. Because we are constantly making
these inferences about people all day long, reading facial
expressions, tone of voice, it'smostly automatic.
(36:44):
Totally automatic. We effortlessly distinguish
between, say, genuine amusement and sarcasm, or nervousness and
guilt, just from tiny fleeting cues.
It's classic thin slicing applied to social interaction.
And the science behind reading faces is fascinating.
People like Sylvan Tompkins, Paul Ekman.
Pioneers Tompkins had this almost uncanny ability.
(37:04):
He could apparently watch silentfilms of people from completely
unfamiliar cultures, like tribesin New Guinea, and accurately
describe their social dynamics, their personalities, sweet and
gentle versus violent and aggressive, just by observing
their facial expressions. He was reading the universal
language of the face. Wow.
Without knowing anything. Else about them without knowing
their language or customs. Yes, and Paul Ekman, his
(37:26):
student, took this further and systemized it.
He developed the facial action coding system FACS, breaking
down every possible facial expression into specific muscle
movements called Action Units orAUS.
So he created like a scientific map of the human face, and it's
linked to emotions. Essentially, yes.
He showed that basic emotions have universal facial
(37:47):
expressions corresponding to specific combinations of AUS.
A genuine smile, for instance, involves not just the mouth
muscles AU 12 but also the muscles around the eyes AU 6,
creating crow's feet. A fake smile often lacks that
eye muscle activation. Maybe Shen smile.
That's the one. And Ekman's core insight is
profound. The face doesn't just signal
(38:08):
emotion, it often is the emotionrevealing our true state, even
when we try to hide it. He talks about micro
expressions, these incredibly brief involuntary flashes of
true feeling that leak out before we can suppress them,
like that suicidal patient Mary,who said she felt better but
showed a fleeting micro expression of utter despair.
It's like the face is a window to the unconscious.
(38:29):
A very revealing 1. And to understand just how
fundamental this non verbal mindreading is, Gladwell discusses
Amy cleanse work with autistic individuals.
He describes Peter, a man with high functioning autism who is
incredibly intelligent verbally but has profound difficulty
reading social cues. He has almost no intuition about
faces or body language. So he has to consciously figure
(38:50):
out what a smile means, like solving a puzzle.
Exactly. He has to logically deduce
social meaning that most of us grasp instantly.
Intuitively, it highlights how much of our social fluency
relies on these unconscious blinks about other people's
states of mind. OK, so mind reading is crucial,
but it failed in the dialogue case.
What role did stress play? A massive role Extreme stress
(39:11):
fundamentally changes how we perceive the world.
Gladwell sites researcher David Clinger, who studies police
officers involved in shootings. They report bizarre perceptual
distortions. Like what?
Things like experiencing events in slow motion, having tunnel
vision where the peripheral awareness just disappears, or
experiencing auditory exclusion,not hearing their own gunshots
(39:33):
or shouts from fellow officers even right next to them.
So stress literally narrows yoursenses.
Drastically, the brain seems to shut down non essential
processing to focus entirely on the perceived threat.
In that state, the ability to pick up subtle social cues to
accurately read some of the intentions from their face or
posture just plummets. The officers in the dialogue
(39:54):
shooting were likely experiencing these intense
perceptual distortions, making accurate mind reading almost
impossible. Their blink was operating with
severely limited stress filtereddata.
And this ties into other police situations, too, like chases.
Yes, the book mentions Bob Martin's analysis of police
pursuits like the Rodney King incident.
High adrenaline, the dog in the hunt mentality can lead officers
(40:17):
to become hyper focused, ignore commands and lose perspective.
And there's also the issue of the false safety of numbers.
Multiple officers rushing in might feel safer, but can
actually escalate situations unnecessarily, leading to
quicker, less considered actions, as potentially happened
in the Robert Rush shooting mentioned.
It sounds like a recipe for disaster if the initial blink is
(40:40):
wrong under stress. It can be, which leads to the
crucial question, can this be fixed?
Can police officers be trained to have better, more reliable
blinks in these high stress situations?
And the answer seems to be yes, based on the work of someone
named Fife. James Feis, Yeah, he did a study
in Dade County, Florida, and found something really
interesting. Officers were actually pretty
bad at following their training during the approach to a
(41:02):
potentially dangerous scene, rushing in, not taking cover,
only adhering to protocol about 15% of the time.
But paradoxically, once they were actually interacting face
to face with suspects, their skills were generally good.
So the problem wasn't the interaction itself, but getting
to the interaction safely and calmly.
That was 5's insight. His crucial message was you
(41:23):
don't want to put yourself in a position where the only way you
have to defend yourself is to shoot someone.
The key wasn't necessarily training officers to shoot
better, but training them to manage the pre encounter phase
better. How?
What kind of training? Training focused on slowing
things down, taking proper cover, calling for backup and
waiting for it, gathering more information before approaching,
(41:45):
coordinating the approach with other officers.
By improving these seemingly mundane procedural behaviors
before the high stress confrontation, they could reduce
the likelihood of situations escalating to the point where
lethal force felt necessary. And did it work?
Dramatically, when Dade County implemented this kind of
training focused on the approach, citizen complaints and
(42:05):
officer injuries dropped significantly.
It showed that good training canfundamentally change the way a
police officer reacts. Not by changing the blink itself
in the heat of the moment, whichis incredibly hard, but by
structuring the situation beforehand to allow for better,
less pressure decision making. So you train the context, you
train the setup to give the blink a better chance of being
(42:27):
right, or to avoid needing a high stakes blink altogether.
Exactly. It's about managing the
conditions under which our rapidcognition operates.
Well, the instincts themselves are automatic.
We can train the behaviors and procedures that surround them to
lead to better outcomes. OK, so training the context is
key, but what about training theintuition itself?
(42:48):
Can we actually make our unconscious blinks smarter, more
accurate over time? Absolutely.
And this really comes down to the power of experience and
deliberate practice. It's about building that rich,
detailed database in your unconscious mind.
Like Thomas Hoeving, the art expert with the Koros, his
instant fake verdict wasn't magic.
Not at all. It was a result of years,
(43:09):
decades of intense immersion. Gladwell describes him spending
countless hours in the Mets storerooms, handling thousands
of objects, looking, touching, comparing.
He wasn't just casually observing.
He was actively pouring and pouring and pouring, absorbing
the subtle details, the feel, the weight, the very essence of
authentic antiquities. So he built this massive
(43:31):
internal library of real. Precisely an unconscious
database. So when he saw the Koros, his
highly trained intuition instantly flagged it as
deviating from that vast internal knowledge base.
It just didn't fit the pattern. His blink was incredibly
accurate because it was built onan incredibly deep foundation of
experience. And it's the same with Gottman
and his marriage predictions. Same principle.
(43:52):
His ability to thin slice of relationship didn't come out of
nowhere. It came from meticulously
analyzing thousands upon thousands of hours of videotape
using his B AFF system to formally code every emotional
nuance. That rigorous structured
analysis honed his ability to recognize the crucial patterns
the relationship fist. Almost instantly, he formalized
(44:12):
his instincts through deep study.
And Victoria Braden, the tennis coach who knew a double fault
was coming but couldn't explain why.
He's another great example. His unconscious mind was picking
up on incredibly subtle tells ina player's motion or body
language cues developed over a lifetime of watching tennis.
But because it was unconscious, he couldn't articulate it.
(44:33):
His project to use bio mechanicsto analyze the movements is an
attempt to bridge that gap, to make the unconscious conscious,
to actually pinpoint the specific data his intuition was
using. So the big idea here is that
expertise isn't about getting rid of intuition, it's about
refining it through focused experience and learning,
building a better unconscious engine.
(44:53):
Exactly. You're not replacing the blink,
you're making it smarter, faster, and more reliable by
feeding at high quality data. Over time, it transforms a fuzzy
gut feeling into a sharp, almostanalytical insight, even if the
analysis itself remains unconscious.
This level of expertise takes serious dedication.
What about everyday intuition? Can just anyone develop expert
level blinks? Well, that's where the limits
(45:15):
come in. Casual exposure isn't enough.
Gladwell uses the Cola Triangle test again to make this point
beautifully. Right where you have 3 cups, two
of one cola, 1 of another, and you have to pick the odd one
out. Yeah.
And as we discussed, even peoplewho think they're cola experts
who drink it every day perform barely better than chance on
this test. They might be OK at telling Coke
(45:37):
from Pepsi head to head, but this finer discrimination task
reveals the limits of their knowledge.
Why is the triangle test so muchharder?
Because simply preferring one taste over another is a basic
first impression, a simple blink.
But identifying the odd one out requires holding the sensory
profiles of all three drinks in your working memory and making
(45:57):
precise comparisons. It demands a more sophisticated
vocabulary of taste, a deeper, more structured understanding
that casual consumption just doesn't provide.
So our everyday knowledge of cola is actually incredibly
shallow, as the book says. Exactly.
We lack the deep, nuanced database that true expertise
requires. Asking our unframed intuition to
(46:18):
perform an expert level task like the triangle test just
reveals its limitations. It highlights the vast
difference between a casual, everyday blink and a finely
tuned expert blank built on years of dedicated experience.
So the take away is intuition istrainable, but it requires real
work, real immersion. You can't just passively absorb
(46:39):
expertise. That seems to be the case.
Focused experience, structured learning, deliberate practice.
That's what transforms basic intuition into reliable expert
judgement. Which leads us beautifully, I
think, to the final really powerful stories in the book,
the ones about blind auditions and orchestras.
Gladwell calls it a small miracle.
It really is a fantastic closingillustration of everything we've
(46:59):
been talking about, bias, context, expertise and how we
can structure things to protect our better judgments.
For years, especially American orchestras, certain sections,
particularly brass sections likehorns and trombones, were almost
most exclusively male domains. Right.
Overwhelmingly so. There was this pervasive, often
unconscious belief, a collectiveblink among conductors and
(47:22):
committees, that women simply didn't have the sound, the
power, the right timber for those instruments.
It was a classic example of bias, like the Warren Harding
error applied to music. They were listening with their
eyes, essentially seeing a womanand assuming she couldn't play
the trombone powerfully. Exactly.
But then, starting around the 70s and 80s, things began to
change. Orchestra started implementing
(47:44):
blind auditions. Screens were put up between the
auditioning musician and the judging committee.
Total anonymity, just the sound.Total anonymity.
Musicians were identified by number.
They even made rules about not wearing shoes that clicked or
walking on carpet to avoid giving away any clues about
gender. The goal was to strip away all
context except the music itself.And the result.
(48:05):
The result was revolutionary. As soon as the screens went up,
the number of women hired by major orchestra started to
skyrocket. Suddenly, talent that had been
overlooked for years, dismissed because of visual bias, was
being recognized. Cladwell tells the story of Abby
Conant, the trombonist a. Powerful story.
She auditioned for the Munich Philharmonic behind a screen in
(48:26):
1980. Played brilliantly, apparently
blew the committee away even though she thought she'd messed
up one note. The music director literally
shouted, that's who we want. Pure musical blink.
They heard greatness. Then she stepped out from behind
the screen and the reaction was shock.
Frau Conant. They were expecting a man, and
(48:47):
even though she won the auditionfair and square based on sound
alone, the visual information, the fact she was a woman,
started to interfere. The ingrained biases resurfaced.
Didn't they try to demote her? They did.
They demoted her, claiming she lacked the necessary physical
strength. Even though her audition proved
her musical superiority, she hadto fight a grueling legal battle
(49:07):
for years to regain her position, proving through
medical tests that her physical capacity was actually superior
to many male players. The screen forced an unbiased
judgement, but removing it allowed the prejudice to creep
back in. And then there's Julie Landsman,
the French horn player. Another incredible story.
She auditioned for the principalhorn position at the Met Opera.
(49:28):
A huge deal, and famously no women in the brass section at
the time the screens were up. She played incredibly
powerfully, nailed a notoriouslydifficult high C, held it
forever. There was no question about her
technical mastery and musicality.
The committee heard it. The blink was undeniable.
Undeniable. She knew she'd won, but when
(49:49):
they announced the winner and she came out from behind the
screen, there was this audible gasp.
Not just because she was a womanplaying with such power, but
because the committee members knew her.
She'd played for them before as a substitute.
But they hadn't realized how good she was until they couldn't
see her. Exactly.
Until the screen removed the visual context, the potential
for bias, they hadn't truly hurther talent.
(50:10):
They'd known her as someone who wasn't principal horn material,
likely influenced by unconsciousbiases.
The screen forced them to listenpurely with their ears and
hearts, not their eyes. That's the small miracle, isn't
it? Creating a situation, a context,
where the Blink judgment is purely about the thing itself,
the music, the talent, stripped bare of all the distorting noise
(50:32):
of prejudice and expectation. It's a perfect encapsulation.
It shows how vulnerable our judgments are to irrelevant
information, but also demonstrates that we can design
systems like the Blind Audition screen to protect our intuition,
to allow the most accurate, unbiased blink to emerge.
Sometimes deliberate blindness leads to clear vision.
(50:52):
So as we wrap up this deep dive,maybe the final thought for
everyone listening is to reflecton that as you go about your
day. How often might you be listening
with your eyes? Or making judgments based on
superficial context rather than substance?
Where might unconscious biases be shaping your blanks about
people, ideas, or opportunities?Yeah.
Where might you be letting a quick, possibly flawed
(51:13):
impression override A deeper insight?
Or on the flip side, where are you maybe over analyzing
something that really just needsa quick intuitive call like
choosing jam? It's about cultivating that
self-awareness, isn't it? Thinking about how you can build
your own expertise through experience like Hobbing or
Gottman, making your intuition more reliable, but also how you
(51:34):
can protect that intuition from distortion, whether it's bias,
information overload, or misleading context.
Maybe even by setting up your own mental screen sometimes.
Because understanding this powerof thinking without thinking,
both its brilliance and its flaws, seems absolutely crucial
for navigating our world more effectively, and maybe more
fairly too. Couldn't dream more.
(51:54):
Well, that's our deep dive into Blink.
Lots to think about there. Thanks so much for joining.
Us always a pleasure. We'll catch you next time for
another deep dive.